- Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon.Michael N. Jones & Douglas J. K. Mewhort - 2007 - Psychological Review 114 (1):1-37.details
|
|
Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.details
|
|
The cognitive RISC machine needs complexity.Richard A. Heath - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):669-670.details
|
|
Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information.James L. McClelland & David E. Rumelhart - 1985 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 114 (2):159-188.details
|
|
Models of verbal working memory capacity: What does it take to make them work?Nelson Cowan, Jeffrey N. Rouder, Christopher L. Blume & J. Scott Saults - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (3):480-499.details
|
|
A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge.Thomas K. Landauer & Susan T. Dumais - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (2):211-240.details
|
|
"Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model.Douglas L. Hintzman - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (4):411-428.details
|
|
Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?S. C. Gandevia & David Burke - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):614-632.details
|
|
Practice and divided attention.William Hirst - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-73.details
|
|
Signal detection with criterion noise: Applications to recognition memory.Aaron S. Benjamin, Michael Diaz & Serena Wee - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (1):84-115.details
|
|
Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern.Ning Lan & Patrick E. Crago - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):769-771.details
|
|
The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.details
|
|
Modular mind or unitary system: A duck-rabbit effect.Gillian Cohen - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):71-72.details
|
|
Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.details
|
|
Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.details
|
|
Inorganic memory.Thomas L. Clarke - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):667-667.details
|
|
Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):655-667.details
|
|
Modeling Recognition Memory Using the Similarity Structure of Natural Input.Joyca P. W. Lacroix, Jaap M. J. Murre, Eric O. Postma & H. Jaap Herik - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (1):121-145.details
|
|
Caught in a bind: Context information and episodic memory.Kevin Murnane - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):675-676.details
|
|
The Maltese cross: Simplistic yes, new no.Thomas H. Carr & Tracy L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):69-71.details
|
|
Why do we need a computational theory of laboratory tasks?Robert L. Greene - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-669.details
|
|
Testing global memory models using ROC curves.Roger Ratcliff, Ching-fan Sheu & Scott D. Gronlund - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (3):518-535.details
|
|
(1 other version)Toward an instance theory of automatization.Gordon D. Logan - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (4):492-527.details
|
|
Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.details
|
|
What kind of a framework?John Morton - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-76.details
|
|
The cerebellum and memory.Richard F. Thompson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):801-802.details
|
|
How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.details
|
|
Brain damage and cognitive dysfunction.Marlene Oscar-Berman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):678-679.details
|
|
Is stiffness the mainspring of posture and movement?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):756-758.details
|
|
Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements?E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi & S. Giszter - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):603-613.details
|
|
Beyond the Tower of Babel in human memory research: The validity and utility of specification.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):682-692.details
|
|
Modeling Recognition Memory Using the Similarity Structure of Natural Input.Joyca P. W. Lacroix, Jaap M. J. Murre, Eric O. Postma & H. Jaap van den Herik - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (1):121-145.details
|
|
A context noise model of episodic word recognition.Simon Dennis & Michael S. Humphreys - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (2):452-478.details
|
|
Modules in models of memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-94.details
|
|
Task-specification language, or theory of human memory?Richard L. Lewis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):674-675.details
|
|
Models of mind: Hidden plumbing.Enoch Callaway - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):68-69.details
|
|
Short-term memory scanning viewed as exemplar-based categorization.Robert M. Nosofsky, Daniel R. Little, Christopher Donkin & Mario Fific - 2011 - Psychological Review 118 (2):280-315.details
|
|
On computational theories and multilevel, multitask models of cognition: The case of word recognition.Arthur M. Jacobs - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):670-672.details
|
|
How does the nervous system control the equilibrium trajectory?S. V. Adamovich - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):704-705.details
|
|
Implications of neural networks for how we think about brain function.David A. Robinson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):644-655.details
|
|
Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.details
|
|
What are the “goals” of the human memory system?David J. Murray - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):676-677.details
|
|
Does a computational theory of human memory need intelligence?Sachiko Kinoshita - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):673-674.details
|
|
Autonomous processing in parallel distributed processing networks.Michael R. W. Dawson & Don P. Schopflocher - 1992 - Philosophical Psychology 5 (2):199-219.details
|
|
Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.details
|
|
The Maltese cross: A new simplistic model for memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):55-68.details
|
|
Marr versus Marr: On the notion of levels.Frank van der Velde, Gezinus Wolters & A. H. C. van der Heijden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):681-682.details
|
|
Can we really dissociate the computational and algorithm-level theories of human memory?Guy Tiberghien - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):680-681.details
|
|
Progress within the bounds of memory.Steven A. Sloman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):679-680.details
|
|
Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.details
|
|