- Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.details
|
|
Editors' Introduction to Networks of the Mind: How Can Network Science Elucidate Our Understanding of Cognition?Thomas T. Hills & Yoed N. Kenett - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (1):189-208.details
|
|
Modules in models of memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-94.details
|
|
Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.details
|
|
How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.details
|
|
Interpolated task characteristics and interference in short-term memory.Charles P. Bird & George E. Weaver - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (1):93-95.details
|
|
Memory and mood.Maryanne Martin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-75.details
|
|
A production system theory of serial memory.John R. Anderson & Michael Matessa - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (4):728-748.details
|
|
Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.details
|
|
Practice and divided attention.William Hirst - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-73.details
|
|
Modular mind or unitary system: A duck-rabbit effect.Gillian Cohen - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):71-72.details
|
|
Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.details
|
|
Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.details
|
|
The Maltese cross: Simplistic yes, new no.Thomas H. Carr & Tracy L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):69-71.details
|
|
What kind of a framework?John Morton - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-76.details
|
|
Models of mind: Hidden plumbing.Enoch Callaway - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):68-69.details
|
|
Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.details
|
|
Short-term proactive interference revisited.A. J. P. Hendrikx - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (5):358-360.details
|
|
The Maltese cross: A new simplistic model for memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):55-68.details
|
|
Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.details
|
|
The use of interference paradigms as a criterion for separating memory stores.Henry L. Roediger - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):78-79.details
|
|
Implicit and explicit memory models.Henry L. Roediger - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 13 (6):339-342.details
|
|
The usefulness for memory theory of the word “store”.D. J. Murray - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):76-77.details
|
|
The homunculus as bureaucrat.Alan K. Mackworth - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):74-74.details
|
|
Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Wisdom, but not especially unconventional.Robert G. Crowder - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-72.details
|
|