Skip to main content
Log in

Anticipating sensitizes the body

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

With emotional motivation the organism prepares the body to obtain a goal. There is an anticipatory sensitization of the sensory systems in the body and the brain. Presynaptic facilitation of the sensory afference in the spinal cord is probably involved. In a second stage the higher centers develop an action image/plan to realize the goal, modifying the initial preparations in the body. The subject experiences the changes in the body as a feeling. Three empirical studies supporting this description are summarized. This description of how feelings develop from emotion circuits is discussed from a phenomenological viewpoint.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. When I refer to feelings, I refer to the experience of feelings, not to tactile sensations. However, when one perceives by touching, there is the feeling experienced as one touches.

  2. Skin conductance is changed by autonomic stimulation to the sweat glands. This changes the galvanic skin response, and is the basis for the somatic marker hypothesis of Damasio (1999, p. 80–81).

References

  • Anscombe, G. E. M. (1957). Intention. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandler, R., & Flynn, J. (1972). Control of somatosensory fields for striking during hypothalamically elicited attack. Brain Research, 38, 197–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnet, M., Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Requin, J. (1997). Mental simulation of an action modulates the excitability of spinal reflex pathways in man. Cognitive Brain Research, 5, 221–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M., Schwartz-Giblin, S., & Pfaff, D. (1985). The pudendal nerve-evoked response in axial muscle. Experimental Brain Research, 61, 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J. (1996). Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 87–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness explained. New York: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • DePreester, H. (2002). Intentionality and the inside/outside distinction in sensitive systems. Consciousness and Emotion, 3, 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (1995). Questioning consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (2000). Efferent brain processes and the enactive approach to consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(4), 40–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (2001). Implications of inattentional blindness for ‘enactive’ theories of consciousness. Brain and Mind, 2, 297–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (2005). Curious emotions. Roots of consciousness and personality in motivated action. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R., & Newton, N. (1998). Three paradoxes of phenomenal consciousness: Bridging the explanatory gap. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(4), 419–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (1995). Body schema and intentionality. In J. Bermudez, A. Marcel, & N. Eilan (Eds.), The body and the self (pp. 225–244). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2000). Exploring the self. A cognitive model of immunity to error through misidentification. In D. Zahavi (Ed.), Philosophical and psychopathological perspectives in self-experience (pp. 203–242). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Jeannerod, M. (2002). From action to interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(1), 3–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2000). The inner sense of action: Agency and motor representations. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(10), 23–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R. (1980). From muscles to motivation. American Scientist, 68, 398–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gandevia, S., Wilson, L., Inglis, J., & Burke, D. (1997). Mental rehearsal of motor tasks recruits alpha-motoneurones, but fails to recruit human fusimotor neurones selectively. Journal of Physiology, 505(1), 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3(3), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14, S103–S109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lethin, A. (2005a). Exposing the covert agent. In R. Ellis, & N. Newton (Eds.), Consciousness and emotion: Agency, conscious choice, and selective perception (pp. 157–180). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lethin, A. (2005b). Covert agency with proprioceptive feedback. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(4/5), 96–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonnell, M., & Flynn, J. (1966a). Control of sensory fields by stimulation of hypothalamus. Science, 152, 1406–1408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonnell, M., & Flynn, J. (1966b). Sensory control of hypothalamic attack. Animal Behavior, 14, 399–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. (2003). The sense of agency: Awareness and ownership of action. In J. Roessler, & N. Eilan (Eds.), Agency and self-awareness (pp. 48–93). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul (translated by C. Smith).

    Google Scholar 

  • Milne, R., Aniss, A., Kay, N., & Gandevia, S. (1988). Reduction in perceived intensity of cutaneous stimuli during movement: A quantitative study. Experimental Brain Research, 70, 569–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J. (1997). Putting the puzzle together, Part II. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(2), 100–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J., & Baars, B. (1993). A neural attentional model for access to consciousness: A global workspace perspective. Concepts in Neuroscience, 4(2), 255–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newton, N. (1996). Foundations of understanding. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newton, N. (2000). The interdependence of consciousness and emotion. Consciousness and Emotion, 1(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newton, N. (2001). Emergence and the uniqueness of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(9–10), 47–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newton, N. (2004). The art of representation: Support for an enactive approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998a). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998b). The periconscious substrates of consciousness: Affective states and the evolutionary origins of the self. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(5–6), 566–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (2000). Commentary on the unconscious Homunculus. Neuropsychoanalysis, 2(1), 24–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., & Watt, D. (2004). The ego is first and foremost a body ego. Neuropsychoanalysis, 5(2), 201–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfaff, D., Schwartz-Giblin, S., McCarthy, M., & Kow, L. (1994). Cellular and molecular mechanisms of female reproductive behaviors. In E. Knobil & J. Neill (Eds.), The physiology of reproduction vol. 2 (pp. 107–221). New York: Raven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Requin, J., Bonnet, M., & Semjen, A. (1977). Is There a specificity in the supraspinal control of motor structures during preparation? In S. Dormic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 139–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, P., & Scott, S. (2001). Sensory-motor control: A long-awaited behavioral correlate of presynaptic inhibition. Nature Neuroscience, 6(12), 1243–1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothwell, J. C. (1995) Critique of papers by Lemon and Bennett, Eyre and Miller, Sessle et al, Taylor et al, and Sears et al. In A. Taylor, M. Gladden, & R. Durbaba (Eds.), Alpha and gamma motor systems, (pp. 379–382). New York: Plenum.

  • Rudomin, P. (2002). Selectivity of the central control of sensory information in the mammalian spinal cord. In S. Gandevia, U. Proske, & D. Stuart (Eds.), Sensorimotor control of movement and posture (pp. 157–169). New York: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudomin, P., & Schmidt, R. (1999). Presynaptic inhibition in the vertebrate spinal cord revisited. Experimental Brain Research, 129(1), 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J., & Freeman, W. (1998). Do we understand consciousness? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(5–6), 718–733.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seki, K., Perlmutter, S., & Fetz, E. (2003). Sensory input to primate spinal cord is presynaptically inhibited during voluntary movement. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1309–1316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanderHorst, V., & Holstege, G. (1996). A concept for the final common pathway of vocalization and lordosis behavior in the cat. In G. Holstege, R. Bandler, & C. Saper (Eds.), The emotional motor system (pp. 327–342). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. (1999). Present-time consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2–3), 111–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., & Depraz, N. (2005). At the source of time. Valence and the constitutional dynamics of affect. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 12(8–10), 61–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S., Fung, S., Jackson, D., & Imel, K. (1996). Serotonin, norepinephrine, and associated neuropeptides: Effects on somatic motoneuron excitability. In G. Holstege, R. Bandler, & C. Saper (Eds.), The emotional motor system (pp. 183–200). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wider, K. (1997). The bodily nature of consciousness. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2003). Husserl’s phenomenology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anton Lethin.

Appendix

Appendix

The gamma motoneurons stimulate the muscle spindles to contract, which increases the proprioceptive afference to the alpha motoneurons and to the brain. This increases the tension or tone in the muscles. During preparation for movement there is presynaptic inhibition of the proprioceptive afference to the alpha motoneurons. It has been suggested that this inhibition may be a method to protect the alpha motoneurons from prematurely stimulating the muscles to contract, while the gamma motor stimulation gets the muscle spindles tensed and ready to vigorously support the alpha motoneurons when the actual movement is initiated (Requin et al. 1977, pp. 139–140; Lethin 2005b, p. 107). There is evidence suggesting that the proprioceptive afference’s pathway to the brain is not inhibited during preparation (Lethin 2005b).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lethin, A. Anticipating sensitizes the body. Phenom Cogn Sci 7, 279–300 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9054-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9054-2

Keywords

Navigation