Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. (1 other version)Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.Richard E. Nisbett & Timothy D. Wilson - 1977 - Psychological Review; Psychological Review 84 (3):231.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   793 citations  
  • The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics.Stanislas Dehaene - 1999 - British Journal of Educational Studies 47 (2):201-203.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   245 citations  
  • Calibrating the mental number line.Véronique Izard & Stanislas Dehaene - 2008 - Cognition 106 (3):1221-1247.
    Human adults are thought to possess two dissociable systems to represent numbers: an approximate quantity system akin to a mental number line, and a verbal system capable of representing numbers exactly. Here, we study the interface between these two systems using an estimation task. Observers were asked to estimate the approximate numerosity of dot arrays. We show that, in the absence of calibration, estimates are largely inaccurate: responses increase monotonically with numerosity, but underestimate the actual numerosity. However, insertion of a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group.Pierre Pica, Cathy Lemer, Véronique Izard & Stanislas Dehaene - 2004 - Science 306 (5695):499-503.
    Is calculation possible without language? Or is the human ability for arithmetic dependent on the language faculty? To clarify the relation between language and arithmetic, we studied numerical cognition in speakers of Mundurukú, an Amazonian language with a very small lexicon of number words. Although the Mundurukú lack words for numbers beyond 5, they are able to compare and add large approximate numbers that are far beyond their naming range. However, they fail in exact arithmetic with numbers larger than 4 (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   173 citations  
  • Exact equality and successor function: Two key concepts on the path towards understanding exact numbers.Véronique Izard, Pierre Pica, Elizabeth S. Spelke & Stanislas Dehaene - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (4):491 – 505.
    Humans possess two nonverbal systems capable of representing numbers, both limited in their representational power: the first one represents numbers in an approximate fashion, and the second one conveys information about small numbers only. Conception of exact large numbers has therefore been thought to arise from the manipulation of exact numerical symbols. Here, we focus on two fundamental properties of the exact numbers as prerequisites to the concept of EXACT NUMBERS : the fact that all numbers can be generated by (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • The origin of concepts.Susan Carey - 2009 - New York: Oxford University Press.
    Only human beings have a rich conceptual repertoire with concepts like tort, entropy, Abelian group, mannerism, icon and deconstruction. How have humans constructed these concepts? And once they have been constructed by adults, how do children acquire them? While primarily focusing on the second question, in The Origin of Concepts , Susan Carey shows that the answers to both overlap substantially. Carey begins by characterizing the innate starting point for conceptual development, namely systems of core cognition. Representations of core cognition (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   481 citations  
  • Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The snarc effect and the reverse snarc effect in English and arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate arabic speakers.Samar Zebian - 2005 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 5 (1-2):165-190.
    The current investigations coordinate math cognition and cultural approaches to numeric thinking to examine the linkages between numeric and spatial processes, and how these linkages are modified by the cultural artifact of writing. Previous research in the adult numeric cognition literature has shown that English monoliterates have a spatialised mental number line which is oriented from left-to-right with smaller magnitudes associated with the left side of space and larger magnitudes are associated with the right side of space. These associations between (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • (1 other version)Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases.Amos Tversky & Daniel Kahneman - 1974 - Science 185 (4157):1124-1131.
    This article described three heuristics that are employed in making judgements under uncertainty: representativeness, which is usually employed when people are asked to judge the probability that an object or event A belongs to class or process B; availability of instances or scenarios, which is often employed when people are asked to assess the frequency of a class or the plausibility of a particular development; and adjustment from an anchor, which is usually employed in numerical prediction when a relevant value (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1712 citations  
  • One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles.Mathieu Le Corre & Susan Carey - 2007 - Cognition 105 (2):395-438.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   102 citations  
  • Nature and culture of finger counting: Diversity and representational effects of an embodied cognitive tool.Andrea Bender & Sieghard Beller - 2012 - Cognition 124 (2):156-182.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Core systems of number.Stanislas Dehaene, Elizabeth Spelke & Lisa Feigenson - 2004 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (7):307-314.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   297 citations  
  • Sixty-four or four-and-sixty? The influence of language and working memory on children's number transcoding.Ineke Imbo, Charlotte Vanden Bulcke, Jolien De Brauwer & Wim Fias - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The mental representation of parity and number magnitude.Stanislas Dehaene, Serge Bossini & Pascal Giraux - 1993 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122 (3):371–96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   264 citations  
  • Children's understanding of counting.Karen Wynn - 1990 - Cognition 36 (2):155-193.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   143 citations  
  • Varieties of numerical abilities.Stanislas Dehaene - 1992 - Cognition 44 (1-2):1-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   116 citations  
  • (1 other version)Preschool Children's Mapping of Number Words to Nonsymbolic Numerosities.Jennifer S. Lipton & Elizabeth S. Spelke - unknown
    Five-year-old children categorized as skilled versus unskilled counters were given verbal estimation and number word comprehension tasks with numerosities 20 – 120. Skilled counters showed a linear relation between number words and nonsymbolic numerosities. Unskilled counters showed the same linear relation for smaller numbers to which they could count, but not for larger number words. Further tasks indicated that unskilled counters failed even to correctly order large number words differing by a 2 : 1 ratio, whereas they performed well on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • The role of understanding in solving word problems.Drzmsra Dellarosa Cummins - unknown
    Word problems are notoriously difficult to solve. We suggest that much of the difficulty children experience with word problems can be attributed to difficulty in comprehending abstract or ambiguous language. We tested this hypothesis by (1) requiring children to recall problems either before or after solving them, (2) requiring them to generate f'mal questions to incomplete word problems, and (3) modeling performance pattems using a computer simulation. Solution performance was found to be systematically related to recall and question generation performance. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Perceptions of perceptual symbols.Lawrence Barsalou - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):637-660.
    Various defenses of amodal symbol systems are addressed, including amodal symbols in sensory-motor areas, the causal theory of concepts, supramodal concepts, latent semantic analysis, and abstracted amodal symbols. Various aspects of perceptual symbol systems are clarified and developed, including perception, features, simulators, category structure, frames, analogy, introspection, situated action, and development. Particular attention is given to abstract concepts, language, and computational mechanisms.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   72 citations  
  • What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms?Noam Sagiv, Julia Simner, James Collins, Brian Butterworth & Jamie Ward - 2006 - Cognition 101 (1):114-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Two steps to space for numbers.Martin H. Fischer & Samuel Shaki - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers.Yi-hui Hung, Daisy L. Hung, Ovid J.-L. Tzeng & Denise H. Wu - 2008 - Cognition 106 (3):1441-1450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Homework: a cross-cultural examination.C. S. Chen & H. W. Stevenson - unknown
    Cultural differences in the amount of time spent on homework and in beliefs and attitudes about homework were investigated through interviews with more than 3,500 elementary school children, their mothers, and their teachers. The children lived in 5 cities: Beijing, Chicago, Minneapolis, Sendai, and Taipei. Chinese children were assigned more homework and spent more time on homework than Japanese children, who in turn were assigned more and spent more time on homework than American children. Chinese children also received more help (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Language and number: a bilingual training study.Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2001 - Cognition 78 (1):45-88.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • A chronometric analysis of simple addition.Guy J. Groen & John M. Parkman - 1972 - Psychological Review 79 (4):329-343.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • Bias in Mental Testing.Arthur R. Jensen - 1981 - British Journal of Educational Studies 29 (2):189-190.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   201 citations  
  • Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to children's learning of multiplication.Patrick Lemaire & Robert S. Siegler - 1995 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124 (1):83.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures.Frank Domahs, Korbinian Moeller, Stefan Huber, Klaus Willmes & Hans-Christoph Nuerk - 2010 - Cognition 116 (2):251-266.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Practical Education.Maria Edgeworth & Richard Lovell Edgeworth - 1815 - Cambridge University Press.
    The scientist Richard Lovell Edgeworth, educated at Trinity College, Dublin, and Oxford, was a Member of the Lunar Society of Birmingham, where he exchanged ideas with other scientists, including James Watt, and was known for his significant mechanical inventions. However, Edgeworth's real interest was education: in this 1788 two-volume work, written with his daughter, the poet Maria Edgeworth, he draws on his own experience of raising twenty children, from which the work derives its authority and innovative character. The work was (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Does ignoring lead to worse evaluations? A new explanation of the stimulus devaluation effect.Kerstin Dittrich & Karl Christoph Klauer - 2012 - Cognition and Emotion 26 (2):193-208.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis.Akira Miyake, Naomi P. Friedman, David A. Rettinger, Priti Shah & Mary Hegarty - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (4):621.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia.Michael McCloskey - 1992 - Cognition 44 (1-2):107-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   67 citations  
  • Cognitive arithmetic across cultures.Jamie I. D. Campbell & Qilin Xue - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (2):299.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Linguistic influences on mathematical development: How important is the transparency of the counting system?Ann Dowker, Sheila Bala & Delyth Lloyd - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (4):523 – 538.
    Wales uses languages with both regular (Welsh) and irregular (English) counting systems. Three groups of 6- and 8-year-old Welsh children with varying degrees of exposure to the Welsh language—those who spoke Welsh at both home and school; those who spoke Welsh only at home; and those who spoke only English—were given standardized tests of arithmetic and a test of understanding representations of two-digit numbers. Groups did not differ on the arithmetic tests, but both groups of Welsh speakers read and compared (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Training to Improve Language Outcomes in Cochlear Implant Recipients.Erin M. Ingvalson & Patrick C. M. Wong - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers.Jonathan I. Flombaum, Justin A. Junge & Marc D. Hauser - 2005 - Cognition 97 (3):315-325.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words.S. Dehaene - 1992 - Cognition 43 (1):1-29.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins.Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Ulrich Weger & Klaus Willmes - 2001 - Cognition 82 (1):B25-B33.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children.Hilary Barth, Kristen La Mont, Jennifer Lipton, Stanislas Dehaene, Nancy Kanwisher & Elizabeth Spelke - 2006 - Cognition 98 (3):199-222.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   63 citations  
  • Language influences number processing – A quadrilingual study.Korbinian Moeller, Samuel Shaki, Silke M. Göbel & Hans-Christoph Nuerk - 2015 - Cognition 136 (C):150-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory. [REVIEW]Mark H. Ashcraft - 1992 - Cognition 44 (1-2):75-106.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Sex differences in the spatial representation of number.Rebecca Bull, Alexandra A. Cleland & Thomas Mitchell - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1):181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Reading space into numbers: a cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect.Samuel Shaki & Martin H. Fischer - 2008 - Cognition 108 (2):590-599.
    Small numbers are spontaneously associated with left space and larger numbers with right space (the SNARC effect), for example when classifying numbers by parity. This effect is often attributed to reading habits but a causal link has so far never been documented. We report that bilingual Russian-Hebrew readers show a SNARC effect after reading Cyrillic script (from left-to-right) that is significantly reduced after reading Hebrew script (from right-to-left). In contrast, they have similar SNARC effects after listening to texts in either (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Minds without language represent number through space: origins of the mental number line.Maria Dolores de Hevia, Luisa Girelli & Viola Macchi Cassia - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Number word structure in first and second language influences arithmetic skills.Anat Prior, Michal Katz, Islam Mahajna & Orly Rubinsten - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Architectures for numerical cognition.Jamie I. D. Campbell - 1994 - Cognition 53 (1):1-44.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children: Ten years later.H. W. Stevenson, C. Chen & S. Y. Lee - unknown
    A decade of heightened emphasis in the United States on mathematics and science education has had little influence on academic achievement or parental attitudes. American elementary school children in 1990 lagged behind their Chinese and Japanese peers to as great a degree as they did in 1980. Comparison of the performance of elementary and secondary school students between 1980 and 1990 reveals a decline from first to eleventh grade in the relative position of American students in mathematics. Parental satisfaction with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: an individual differences approach.Priti Shah & Akira Miyake - 1996 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125 (1):4.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction.Camilla K. Gilmore, Shannon E. McCarthy & Elizabeth S. Spelke - unknown
    Symbolic arithmetic is fundamental to science, technology and economics, but its acquisition by children typically requires years of effort, instruction and drill1,2. When adults perform mental arithmetic, they activate nonsymbolic, approximate number representations3,4, and their performance suffers if this nonsymbolic system is impaired5. Nonsymbolic number representations also allow adults, children, and even infants to add or subtract pairs of dot arrays and to compare the resulting sum or difference to a third array, provided that only approximate accuracy is required6–10. Here (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it.Barbara W. Sarnecka & Susan Carey - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):662-674.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   64 citations  
  • From one to 1: An analysis of a transcoding process by means of neuropsychological data.Gerard Deloche & Xavier Seron - 1982 - Cognition 12 (2):119-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations