Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Common ground.Robert Stalnaker - 2002 - Linguistics and Philosophy 25 (5-6):701-721.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   484 citations  
  • The Theory of Social and Economic Organization.Max Weber, A. M. Henderson & Talcott Parsons - 1947 - Philosophical Review 57 (5):524-528.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   378 citations  
  • Establishing joint decisions in a dyad.Melisa Stevanovic - 2012 - Discourse Studies 14 (6):779-803.
    This study analyses joint decisions. Drawing on video-recorded planning meetings in a workplace context as data, and on conversation analysis as a method, I investigate what is needed for a proposal to get turned into a joint decision: How do people negotiate the outcome of the decision-making processes in terms of whether they indeed comprise new decisions and whether these decisions are really joint ones? This study identifies three essential components in arriving at joint decisions, and discusses two other possible (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Dominance: An alternative view.Barbara Smuts - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):448-449.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Review of John R. Searle: The Construction of Social Reality[REVIEW]Alan Nelson - 1995 - Ethics 108 (1):208-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   304 citations  
  • Opening up Closings.Emanuel A. Schegloff & Harvey Sacks - 1973 - Semiotica 8 (4).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   203 citations  
  • Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights.Federico Rossano, Hannes Rakoczy & Michael Tomasello - 2011 - Cognition 121 (2):219-227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • Situated normativity: The normative aspect of embodied cognition in unreflective action.Erik Rietveld - 2008 - Mind 117 (468):973-1001.
    In everyday life we often act adequately, yet without deliberation. For instance, we immediately obtain and maintain an appropriate distance from others in an elevator. The notion of normativity implied here is a very basic one, namely distinguishing adequate from inadequate, correct from incorrect, or better from worse in the context of a particular situation. In the first part of this paper I investigate such ‘situated normativity’ by focusing on unreflective expert action. More particularly, I use Wittgenstein’s examples of craftsmen (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   127 citations  
  • On being the object of attention: Implications for self-other consciousness.Vasudevi Reddy - 2003 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (9):397-402.
    Joint attention to an external object at the end of the first year is typically believed to herald the infant's discovery of other people's attention. I will argue that mutual attention in the first months of life already involves an awareness of the directednesss of attention. The self is experienced as the first object of this directedness followed by gradually more distal 'objects'. this view explains early infant affective self-consciousness within mutual attention as emotionally meaningful, rather than as bearing only (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   68 citations  
  • The interaction order Sui generis: Goffman's contribution to social theory.Anne Warfield Rawls - 1987 - Sociological Theory 5 (2):136-149.
    Goffman is credited with enriching our understanding of the details of interaction, but not with challenging our theoretical understanding of social organization. While Goffman's position is not consistent, the outlines for a theory of an interaction order sui generis may be found in his work. It is not theoretically adequate to understand Goffman as an interactionist within the dichotomy between agency and social structure. Goffman offers a way of resolving this dichotomy via the idea of an interaction order which is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Compliance, resistance and incipient compliance when responding to directives.Alexandra Kent - 2012 - Discourse Studies 14 (6):711-730.
    How does a parent get a child to do something? And, indeed, how might the child avoid complying or seem to comply without actually having done so? This article uses conversation analysis to identify the interactionally preferred and dispreferred response to directives. It then focuses on one alternative response option that has both verbal and embodied elements. The first part involves an embodied display of incipient compliance. That is, actions that are preparatory steps towards compliance and signal that it may (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Action formation and its epistemic (and other) backgrounds.John Heritage - 2013 - Discourse Studies 15 (5):551-578.
    This article reviews arguments that, in the process of action formation and ascription, the relative status of the participants with respect to a projected action can adjust or trump the action stance conveyed by the linguistic form of the utterance. In general, congruency between status and stance is preferred, and linguistic form is a fairly reliable guide to action ascription. However incongruities between stance and status result in action ascriptions that are at variance with the action stance that is otherwise (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Central Problems in Social Theory: Action, Structure and Contradiction in Social Analysis.Anthony Giddens - 1980 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 41 (1):246-247.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   172 citations  
  • Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition.Hanne De Jaegher & Ezequiel Di Paolo - 2007 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 6 (4):485-507.
    As yet, there is no enactive account of social cognition. This paper extends the enactive concept of sense-making into the social domain. It takes as its departure point the process of interaction between individuals in a social encounter. It is a well-established finding that individuals can and generally do coordinate their movements and utterances in such situations. We argue that the interaction process can take on a form of autonomy. This allows us to reframe the problem of social cognition as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   398 citations  
  • Language and Symbolic Power.Ian Buchanan, Pierre Bourdieu, Gino Raymond & Matthew Adamson - 1993 - Substance 22 (2/3):342.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   462 citations  
  • The Role of Interaction Formats in Language Acquisition.Jerome Bruner - 1985 - In J. P. Forgas (ed.), Language and Social Situations. New York:
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Understanding social norms and constitutive rules: Perspectives from developmental psychology and philosophy.Ingar Brinck - 2015 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 14 (4):699-718.
    An experimental paradigm that purports to test young children’s understanding of social norms is examined. The paradigm models norms on Searle’s notion of a constitutive rule. The experiments and the reasons provided for their design are discussed. It is argued that the experiments do not provide direct evidence about the development of social norms and that the concepts of a social norm and constitutive rule are distinct. The experimental data are re-interpreted, and suggestions for how to deal with the present (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Rhythm in telephone closings.Peter Auer - 1990 - Human Studies 13 (4):361 - 392.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The Construction of Social Reality.John Searle - 1995 - Philosophy 71 (276):313-315.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   493 citations  
  • Talk in Action: Interactions, Identities, and Institutions.[author unknown] - 2010
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • Analysing Interactions in Childhood: Insights from Conversation Analysis.[author unknown] - 2010
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Imperative Turns at Talk.[author unknown] - 2017
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Mind, self and society.George H. Mead - 1934 - Chicago, Il.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   878 citations  
  • Developing an understanding of social norms and games : Emotional engagement, nonverbal agreement, and conversation.Ingar Brinck - 2014 - Theory and Psychology 24 (6):737–754.
    The first part of the article examines some recent studies on the early development of social norms that examine young children’s understanding of codified rule games. It is argued that the constitutive rules than define the games cannot be identified with social norms and therefore the studies provide limited evidence about socio-normative development. The second part reviews data on children’s play in natural settings that show that children do not understand norms as codified or rules of obligation, and that the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The role of intersubjectivity in intentional communication.Ingar Brinck - 2008 - In Tim Racine, Jordan Zlatev, Chris Sinha & Esa Itkonen (eds.), The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity.
    The present account explains which elements of the act of nonverbal reference are intersubjective, which major effects intersubjectivity has on the development of intentional communication and at what stages, and how intersubjectivity contributes to trigger the general capacity for nonverbal reference in the second year. First, intersubjectivity is analysed in terms of a mutual or individual, dyadic or triadic, sharing of experiences. It is then shown that nonverbal reference presupposes intersubjectivity relative to communicative-intent indicating and referential behaviour, and the modification (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The role of intersubjectivity for the development of intentional communication.Ingar Brinck - 2008 - In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha & E. Itkonen (eds.), The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity. John Benjamins. pp. 115--140.
    The present account explains (i) which elements of nonverbal reference are intersubjective, (ii) what major effects intersubjectivity has on the general development of intentional communication and at what stages, and (iii) how intersubjectivity contributes to triggering the general capacity for nonverbal reference in the second year of life. First, intersubjectivity is analysed in terms of a sharing of experiences that is either mutual or individual, and either dyadic or triadic. Then it is shown that nonverbal reference presupposes intersubjectivity in communicative (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The Nature and Limits of Authority.R. T. DeGEORGE - 1985
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Authority.Richard S. Peters - 1967 - In Anthony Quinton (ed.), Political Philosophy. London: Oxford University Press. pp. 83--96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Authority'in A. Quinton.R. S. Peters - 1967 - In Anthony Quinton (ed.), Political Philosophy. London: Oxford University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Appeal to Expert Opinion: Arguments from Authority.Douglas Walton - 1999 - Philosophy 74 (289):454-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • Power: A Radical View.Steven Lukes & Jack H. Nagel - 1976 - Political Theory 4 (2):246-249.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   246 citations