Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?Keith E. Stanovich & Richard F. West - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5):645-665.
    Much research in the last two decades has demonstrated that human responses deviate from the performance deemed normative according to various models of decision making and rational judgment (e.g., the basic axioms of utility theory). This gap between the normative and the descriptive can be interpreted as indicating systematic irrationalities in human cognition. However, four alternative interpretations preserve the assumption that human behavior and cognition is largely rational. These posit that the gap is due to (1) performance errors, (2) computational (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   376 citations  
  • The nature and function of consciousness: Lessons from blindsight.Guven Guzeldere, Owen J. Flanagan & Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 2000 - In Michael S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences: 2nd Edition. MIT Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Perception without awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology.Philip M. Merikle & Daniel Smilek - 2001 - Cognition 79 (1):115-34.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   105 citations  
  • Reclaiming volition: An alternative interpretation of Libet's experiment.Jing Zhu - 2003 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 10 (11):61-77.
    Based on his experimental studies, Libet claims that voluntary actions are initiated by unconscious brain activities well before intentions or decisions to act are consciously experienced by people. This account conflicts with our common-sense conception of human agency, in which people consciously and intentionally exert volitions or acts of will to initiate voluntary actions. This paper offers an alternative interpretation of Libet's experiment. The cause of the intentional acts performed by the subjects in Libet's experiment should not be exclusively attributed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework.Stanislas Dehaene & Lionel Naccache - 2001 - Cognition 79 (1):1-37.
    This introductory chapter attempts to clarify the philosophical, empirical, and theoretical bases on which a cognitive neuroscience approach to consciousness can be founded. We isolate three major empirical observations that any theory of consciousness should incorporate, namely (1) a considerable amount of processing is possible without consciousness, (2) attention is a prerequisite of consciousness, and (3) consciousness is required for some specific cognitive tasks, including those that require durable information maintenance, novel combinations of operations, or the spontaneous generation of intentional (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   456 citations  
  • Precis of the modularity of mind.Jerry A. Fodor - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):1-42.
    The Modularity of Mind proposes an alternative to the or view of cognitive architecture that has dominated several decades of cognitive science. Whereas interactionism stresses the continuity of perceptual and cognitive processes, modularity theory argues for their distinctness. It is argued, in particular, that the apparent plausibility of New Look theorizing derives from the failure to distinguish between the (correct) claim that perceptual processes are inferential and the (dubious) claim that they are unencapsidated, that is, that they are arbitrarily sensitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   278 citations  
  • The attention system of the human brain.Michael I. Posner & Steven E. Petersen - 1990 - Annual Review of Neuroscience 13:25-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   216 citations  
  • Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory.James L. McClelland, Bruce L. McNaughton & Randall C. O'Reilly - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (3):419-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   220 citations  
  • CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition.John Morton & Mark H. Johnson - 1991 - Psychological Review 98 (2):164-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Analysis of aversive memories using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm.M. Davis - 1992 - In Neuropsychology of Memory. pp. 470--484.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Cognitive-Emotional Interactions in the Brain.Joseph E. Ledoux - 1989 - Cognition and Emotion 3 (4):267-289.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior.D. Norman & T. Shallice - 1986 - In R. Davidson, R. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory IV. Plenum Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   222 citations  
  • From an animal's point of view: Motivation, fitness, and animal welfare.Marian Stamp Dawkins - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):1-9.
    To study animal welfare empirically we need an objective basis for deciding when an animal is suffering. Suffering includes a wide range ofunpleasant emotional states such as fear, boredom, pain, and hunger. Suffering has evolved as a mechanism for avoiding sources ofdanger and threats to fitness. Captive animals often suffer in situations in which they are prevented from doing something that they are highly motivated to do. The an animal is prepared to pay to attain or to escape a situation (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   80 citations  
  • (2 other versions)Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention.Walter Schneider & Richard M. Shiffrin - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (1):1-66.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   593 citations  
  • Dissociable learning and memory systems of the brain.Larry R. Squire, Stephan Hamann & Barbara Knowlton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):422-423.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • (1 other version)Damasio's error?Jaak Panksepp - 2003 - Consciousness and Emotion 4 (1):111-134.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention.O. Neumann - 1987 - In H. Heuer & H. F. Sanders (eds.), Perspectives on Perception and Action. Lawerence Erlbaum.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Abnormalities in the awareness of action.Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Daniel M. Wolpert & Christopher D. Frith - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (6):237-242.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   184 citations  
  • Classical conditioning and brain systems: The role of awareness.Robert E. D. Clark & L. R. Squire - 1998 - Science 280:77-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   65 citations  
  • The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence.Larry L. Jacoby, Andrew P. Yonelinas & J. M. Jennings - 1997 - In Jonathan D. Cohen & Jonathan W. Schooler (eds.), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 13--47.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Attentional mechanisms and conscious experience.Michael I. Posner & M. K. Rothbart - 1991 - In A. David Milner & M. D. Rugg (eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness. Academic Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks.Stanislas Dehaene, Michel Kerszberg & Jean-Pierre Changeux - 2001 - Pnas 95 (24):14529-14534.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   90 citations  
  • Deficit studies and the function of phenomenal consciousness.Robert van Gulick - 1994 - In George Graham & G. Lynn Stephens (eds.), Philosophical Psychopathology. MIT Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning.Daniel B. Willingham - 1998 - Psychological Review 105 (3):558-584.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • The evolution of multiple memory systems.David F. Sherry & Daniel L. Schacter - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):439-454.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   189 citations  
  • The empirical case for two systems of reasoning.Steven A. Sloman - 1996 - Psychological Bulletin 119 (1):3-22.
    Distinctions have been proposed between systems of reasoning for centuries. This article distills properties shared by many of these distinctions and characterizes the resulting systems in light of recent findings and theoretical developments. One system is associative because its computations reflect similarity structure and relations of temporal contiguity. The other is "rule based" because it operates on symbolic structures that have logical content and variables and because its computations have the properties that are normally assigned to rules. The systems serve (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   466 citations  
  • The comparative psychology of intelligence.Euan M. Macphail - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):645.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   193 citations  
  • Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach.Matthew D. Lieberman - 2000 - Psychological Bulletin 126 (1):109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.M. Corbetta & G. L. Shulman - 2002 - Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3 (3):201-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   392 citations  
  • Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action.Scott Glover - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (1):3-24.
    Evidence for a dichotomy between the planning of an action and its on-line control in humans is reviewed. This evidence suggests that planning and control each serve a specialized purpose utilizing distinct visual representations. Evidence from behavioral studies suggests that planning is influenced by a large array of visual and cognitive information, whereas control is influenced solely by the spatial characteristics of the target, including such things as its size, shape, orientation, and so forth. Evidence from brain imaging and neuropsychology (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   61 citations  
  • Where brain, body, and world collide.Andy Clark - 1999 - Cognitive Systems Research 1 (1):5--17.
    --œWhere Brain, Body, and World Collide--� reprinted by permission of Daedalus, Journal of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, from the issue entitled, --œThe Brain,--� Spring 1998, Vol. 127, No. 2.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Visual awareness and visuomotor action.Andy Clark - 1999 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 6 (11-12):1-18.
    Recent work in "embodied, embedded" cognitive science links mental contents to large-scale distributed effects: dynamic patterns implicating elements of (what are traditionally seen as) sensing, reasoning and acting. Central to this approach is an idea of biological cognition as profoundly "action-oriented" - geared not to the creation of rich, passive inner models of the world, but to the cheap and efficient production of real-world action in real-world context. A case in point is Hurley's (1998) account of the profound role of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • From an implicit to an explicit "theory of mind".Josef Perner & W. Clements - 2000 - In Yves Rossetti (ed.), Beyond Dissociation: Interaction Between Dissociated Implicit and Explicit Processing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious.S. Epstein - 1994 - American Psychologist 49 (8):409-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • Brain physiology and the unconscious initiation of movements.R. Näätänen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):549-549.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Associations across time: The hippocampus as a temporary memory store.J. N. P. Rawlins - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):479-497.
    All recent memory theories of hippocampal function have incorporated the idea that the hippocampus is required to process items only of some qualitatively specifiahle kind, and is not required to process items of some complementary set. In contrast, it is now proposed that the hippocampus is needed to process stimuli of all kinds, but only when there is a need to associate those stimuli with other events that are temporally discontiguous. In order to form or use temporally discontiguous associations, it (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   142 citations  
  • Evidence of divergence in vertebrate learning.M. E. Bitterman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):659.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Some contributions of neuropsychology of vision and memory to the problem of consciousness.Lawrence Weiskrantz - 1988 - In Anthony J. Marcel & Edoardo Bisiach (eds.), Consciousness in Contemporary Science. New York: Oxford University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief.Vinod Goel & Raymond J. Dolan - 2003 - Cognition 87 (1):B11-B22.
    Although deductive reasoning is a closed system, one's beliefs about the world can influence validity judgements. To understand the associated functional neuroanatomy of this belief-bias we studied 14 volunteers using event-related fMRI, as they performed reasoning tasks under neutral, facilitatory and inhibitory belief conditions. We found evidence for the engagement of a left temporal lobe system during belief-based reasoning and a bilateral parietal lobe system during belief-neutral reasoning. Activation of right lateral prefrontal cortex was evident when subjects inhibited a prepotent (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Context, cortex, and dopamine: A connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia.Jonathan D. Cohen & David Servan-Schreiber - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (1):45-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   110 citations  
  • Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex.Peter J. Lang, Margaret M. Bradley & Bruce N. Cuthbert - 1990 - Psychological Review 97 (3):377-395.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective.Arthur S. Reber - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (2):93-133.
    In recent decades it has become increasingly clear that a substantial amount of cognitive work goes on independent of consciousness. The research has been carried out largely under two rubrics, implicit learning and implicit memory. The former has been concerned primarily with the acquisition of knowledge independent of awareness and the latter with the manner in which memories not readily available to conscious recall or recognition play a role in behavior; collectively these operations comprise the essential functions of the cognitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   104 citations  
  • Evolutionary psychology -- towards a more integrative model.Frederick Toates - 2005 - Biology and Philosophy 20 (2-3):305-328.
    Aspects of the history of behavioural science are reviewed, pointing to its fragmented and faction-ridden nature. The emergence of evolutionary psychology (EP) is viewed in this context. With the help of a dual-layered model of behavioural control, the case is made for a more integrative perspective towards EP. The model's application to both behaviour and complex human information processing is described. Similarities in their control are noted. It is suggested that one layer of control (‘on-line’) corresponds to the encapsulated modules (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Chunking and consolidation: A theoretical synthesis of semantic networks, configuring in conditioning, S-R versus cognitive learning, normal forgetting, the amnesic syndrome, and the hippocampal arousal system.Wayne A. Wickelgren - 1979 - Psychological Review 86 (1):44-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   210 citations  
  • Associative learning and the cognitive map: Differences in intelligence as expressions of a common learning mechanism.Stephen Kaplan - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):672.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Dual functions of consciousness.Tim Shallice - 1972 - Psychological Review 79 (5):383-93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   139 citations  
  • Hippocampus, space, and memory.David S. Olton, James T. Becker & Gail E. Handelmann - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):313-322.
    We examine two different descriptions of the behavioral functions of the hippocampal system. One emphasizes spatially organized behaviors, especially those using cognitive maps. The other emphasizes memory, particularly working memory, a short-term memory that requires iexible stimulus-response associations and is highly susceptible to interference. The predictive value of the spatial and memory descriptions were evaluated by testing rats with damage to the hippocampal system in a series of experiments, independently manipulating the spatial and memory characteristics of a behavioral task. No (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   292 citations  
  • Consciousness as a message-aware control mechanism to modulate cognitive processing.Walter E. Schneider & M. Pimm-Smith - 1997 - In Jonathan D. Cohen & Jonathan W. Schooler (eds.), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 65--80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What qualifies a representation for a role in consciousness?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1997 - In Jonathan D. Cohen & Jonathan W. Schooler (eds.), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness. Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Whodunnit? Unpicking the 'seems' of free will.Guy Claxton - 1999 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 6 (8-9):8-9.
    The cornerstone of the dominant folk theory of free will is the presumption that conscious intentions are, at least sometimes, causally related to subsequent ‘voluntary’ actions. Like all folk theories that have become ‘second nature', this model skews perception and cognition to highlight phenomena and interpretations that are consistent with itself, and pathologize or render invisible those that are not. A variety of experimental, neurological and everyday phenomena are reviewed that cumulatively cast doubt on this comforting folk model. An alternative (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations