Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. An evolutionary analysis of rules regulating human inbreeding and marriage.Nancy Wilmsen Thornhill - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):247-261.
    Evolutionary theory predicts that humans should avoid incest because of the negative effects incest has on individual reproduction: production of defective offspring. Selection for the avoidance of close-kin mating has apparently resulted in a psychological mechanism that promotes voluntary incest avoidance. Most human societies are thought to have rules regulating incest. If incest is avoided, why are social rules constructed to regulate it? This target article suggests that incest rules do not exist primarily to regulate close-kin mating but to regulate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Muddled theory and misinterpreted data: Comments on yet another attempt to identify a so-called Westermarck effect and, in the process, to refute Freud.David H. Spain - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):278-279.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Beyond the Westermarck effect: The role of denial and nurturant bonding in incest avoidance.Karin C. Meiselman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):274-275.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Multiple causes, eye witnesses and imaginative fertility.Ray H. Bixler - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):265-266.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The derealization of rape.Betty M. Bayer & Robert S. Steele - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):380-381.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolutionary psychology of men's coercive sexuality.Randy Thornhill & Nancy Wilmsen Thornhill - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):363-375.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   131 citations  
  • Social versus reproductive success: The central theoretical problem of human sociobiology.Daniel R. Vining - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):167-187.
    The fundamental postulate of sociobiology is that individuals exploit favorable environments to increase their genetic representation in the next generation. The data on fertility differentials among contemporary humans are not cotvietent with this postulate. Given the importance ofHomo sapiensas an animal species in the natural world today, these data constitute particularly challenging and interesting problem for both human sociobiology and sociobiology as a whole.The first part of this paper reviews the evidence showing an inverse relationship between reproductive fitness and “endowment” (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Confessions of a curmudgeon.Philip Kitcher - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):89-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Is there really “juggling,” “artifice,” and “trickery” in Genes, Mind, and Culture?Alexander Rosenberg - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):80-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Optimist/pessimist.Elliott Sober - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):87-88.
    The reception so far of Kitcher's Vaulting Ambition reminds me of the old saw about the difference between an optimist and a pessimist. Looking at the same glass of water, the former sees it as half full while the latter sees it as half empty. Some have seen Kitcher's book as a vindication of the possibility of an evolutionary science of human behavior; others have seen it as a devastating critique of the most influential efforts to date to construct such (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavior genetics moves beyond percentages – at last.Robert J. Sternberg - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):40-40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The problem of documenting systematic nonshared environmental effects directly.Kathleen McCartney - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):32-33.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pop goes the weasel.Alex Walter - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):185-186.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The meaning of “evolutionary law”.L. B. Slobodkin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):252-253.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mate preference is not mate selection.Ada Zohar & Ruth Guttman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):38-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Homo sapiens: A good fit to theory, but posing some enigmas.Janet L. Leonard - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):26-27.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures.David M. Buss - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):1-14.
    Contemporary mate preferences can provide important clues to human reproductive history. Little is known about which characteristics people value in potential mates. Five predictions were made about sex differences in human mate preferences based on evolutionary conceptions of parental investment, sexual selection, human reproductive capacity, and sexual asymmetries regarding certainty of paternity versus maternity. The predictions centered on how each sex valued earning capacity, ambition— industriousness, youth, physical attractiveness, and chastity. Predictions were tested in data from 37 samples drawn from (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   316 citations  
  • A psychologist's perspective on incest avoidance behavior.Karin C. Meiselman - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):112-112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarity and ethnicity mediate human relationships, but why?J. Philippe Rushton - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):548-559.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Adaptation and natural selection: A new look at some old ideas.Jeffry A. Simpson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):634-636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Group selection: The theory replaces the bogey man.David Sloan Wilson & Elliott Sober - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):639-654.
    In both biology and the human sciences, social groups are sometimes treated as adaptive units whose organization cannot be reduced to individual interactions. This group-level view is opposed by a more individualistic one that treats social organization as a byproduct of self-interest. According to biologists, group-level adaptations can evolve only by a process of natural selection at the group level. Most biologists rejected group selection as an important evolutionary force during the 1960s and 1970s but a positive literature began to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences.David Sloan Wilson & Elliott Sober - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):585-608.
    In both biology and the human sciences, social groups are sometimes treated as adaptive units whose organization cannot be reduced to individual interactions. This group-level view is opposed by a more individualistic one that treats social organization as a byproduct of self-interest. According to biologists, group-level adaptations can evolve only by a process of natural selection at the group level. Most biologists rejected group selection as an important evolutionary force during the 1960s and 1970s but a positive literature began to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   135 citations  
  • On the biology and politics of cognitive sex differences.David C. Geary - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):267-284.
    The male advantage in certain mathematical domains contributes to the difference in the numbers of males and females that enter math-intensive occupations, which in turn contributes to the sex difference in earnings. Understanding the nature and development of the sex difference in mathematical abilities is accordingly of social as well as scientific concern. A more complete understanding of the biological contributions to these differences can guide research on educational techniques that might someday produce more equal educational outcomes in mathematics and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does consequentialism pay?Adam Morton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):24-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sexual selection and sex differences in mathematical abilities.David C. Geary - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):229-247.
    The principles of sexual selection were used as an organizing framework for interpreting cross-national patterns of sex differences in mathematical abilities. Cross-national studies suggest that there are no sex differences in biologically primary mathematical abilities, that is, for those mathematical abilities that are found in all cultures as well as in nonhuman primates, and show moderate heritability estimates. Sex differences in several biologically secondary mathematical domains are found throughout the industrialized world. In particular, males consistently outperform females in the solving (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The logical relation between cultural and biological evolution: On to the next question.Jerome H. Barkow - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):235-236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?John Archer - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):249-266.
    I argue that the magnitude and nature of sex differences in aggression, their development, causation, and variability, can be better explained by sexual selection than by the alternative biosocial version of social role theory. Thus, sex differences in physical aggression increase with the degree of risk, occur early in life, peak in young adulthood, and are likely to be mediated by greater male impulsiveness, and greater female fear of physical danger. Male variability in physical aggression is consistent with an alternative (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • False beliefs and naive beliefs: They can be good for you.Roberto Casati & Marco Bertamini - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (6):512-513.
    Naive physics beliefs can be systematically mistaken. They provide a useful test-bed because they are common, and also because their existence must rely on some adaptive advantage, within a given context. In the second part of the commentary we also ask questions about when a whole family of misbeliefs should be considered together as a single phenomenon.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Science education and moral education.Holmes Rolston - 1988 - Zygon 23 (3):347-355.
    Both science and ethics are embedded in cultural traditions where truths are shared through education; both need competent critics educated within such traditions. Education in both ought to be directed although moral education demands levels of responsible agency that science education does not. Evolutionary science often carries an implicit or explicit understanding of who and what humans are, one which may not be coherent with the implicit or explicit human self‐understanding in moral education. The latter in turn may not be (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Visiting dead ancestors: Shamans as interpreters of religious traditions.Lyle B. Steadman & Craig T. Palmer - 1994 - Zygon 29 (2):173-189.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Wat is menselijk? Wat is wenselijk?Pouwel Slurink - 2006 - Krisis 7 (1):26-41.
    Relatively short Dutch introduction to an evolutionary approach to morality. A synthesis is given of various models of moral evolution. Some remarks are made on a way to look at the evolution of a compatibilistic 'free will' and a model is given of a way in which the 'good' can be understood as the results of shared interests (which, of course, gives an incomplete model, but at the same time throws a lot of light on the way in which we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ethology, sociobiology and evolutionary psychology.Paul Edmund Griffiths - 2008 - In Sahorta Sarkar & Anya Plutynski (eds.), Companion to the Philosophy of Biology. Blackwell. pp. 393-414.
    In the years leading up to the Second World War the ethologists Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen, created the tradition of rigorous, Darwinian research on animal behavior that developed into modern behavioral ecology. At first glance, research on specifically human behavior seems to exhibit greater discontinuity that research on animal behavior in general. The 'human ethology' of the 1960s appears to have been replaced in the early 1970s by a new approach called ‘sociobiology’. Sociobiology in its turn appears to have (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • E pluribus unum?Daniel C. Dennett - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):617-618.
    W&S correctly ask if groups can be like individuals in the harmony and cooperation of their parts, but in their answer, they ignore the importance of the difference between genetically related and unrelated components, and also misconstrue the import of the Hutterites.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The rectification of names.David Edward Shaner - 1987 - Biology and Philosophy 2 (3):347-368.
    The beginning of any rigorous interdisciplinary study, as Hegel and later Marx predicted, is going to be the occasion for opposition, contradiction, negation and mediation. Sociobiology is not a mature field (thesis). Kitcher's critical work entitledVaulting Ambition seeks to at once expose the failings of this field (serving as antithesis) while simultaneously defining the requirements for more mature, and thus epistemologically satisfying, sociobiological explanations (synthesis). The sociobiological research agenda is thus implicitly given a green light provided certain methodological precautions are (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Critical notice.Kim Sterelny - 1988 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 66 (4):538 – 555.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Dancing in the dark: Evolutionary psychology and the argument from design.Karola Stotz & Paul E. Griffiths - 2002 - In Steven J. Scher & Frederick Rauscher (eds.), Evolutionary Psychology: Alternative Approaches. Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 135--160.
    The Narrow Evolutionary Psychology Movement represents itself as a major reorientation of the social/behavioral sciences, a group of sciences previously dominated by something called the ‘Standard Social Science Model’. Narrow Evolutionary Psychology alleges that the SSSM treated the mind, and particularly those aspects of the mind that exhibit cultural variation, as devoid of any marks of its evolutionary history. Adherents of Narrow Evolutionary Psychology often suggest that the SSSM owed more to ideology than to evidence. It was the child of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • De kloof tussen zin en zijn. Darwinisme, doelen en ons zoeken naar zin.Pouwel Slurink - 1993 - In Ria van den Brandt (ed.), Het heil van de filosofie. Ambo. pp. 116-147.
    Philosophical questions can often be answered using evolutionary biology and evolutionary psychology. Of course, one needs a sound epistemology and philosophy os science to do so. Phenomenology and hermeneutics offer no escape route, however, because they are based on a wrong model of science. Evolutionary biology can explain teleology, the organization of nature, altruïsm, morality, and even our quest for meaning.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sex differences in behavioral and hormonal response to social threat: Commentary on Taylor et al. (2000).David C. Geary & Mark V. Flinn - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (4):745-750.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The adaptive value associated with expressing and perceiving angry-male and happy-female faces.Peter Kay Chai Tay - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Men are not born to rape.Andrew Futterman & Sabrina Zirkel - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):385-386.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rising out of the ashes.H. C. Plotkin - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):79-80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Darwin and human nature.Donald Symons - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):89-89.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Précis of Vaulting Ambition: Sociobiology and the Quest for Human Nature.Philip Kitcher - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):61-71.
    The debate about the credentials of sociobiology has persisted because scholars have failed to distinguish the varieties of sociobiology and because too little attention has been paid to the details of the arguments that are supposed to support the provocative claims about human social behavior. I seek to remedy both deficiencies. After analysis of the relationships among different kinds of sociobiology and contemporary evolutionary theory, I attempt to show how some of the studies of the behavior of nonhuman animals meet (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • Children in the same family are very different, but why?Robert Plomin & Denise Daniels - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):44-59.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Distinctive environments depend on genotypes.Sandra Scarr - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):38-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Why are children in the same family so different from one another?Robert Plomin & Denise Daniels - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):1-16.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   234 citations  
  • On constraints and adaptation.R. C. Lewontin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):244-245.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Evolution and populations.Paul C. Mundinger - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):245-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A multiple-level model of evolution and its implications for sociobiology.H. C. Plotkin & F. J. Odling-Smee - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):225-235.
    The fundamental tenet of contemporary sociobiology, namely the assumption of a single process of evolution involving the selection of genes, is critically examined. An alternative multiple-level, multiple-process model of evolution is presented which posits that the primary process that operates via selection upon the genes cannot account for certain kinds of biological phenomena, especially complex, learned, social behaviours. The primary process has evolved subsidiary evolutionary levels and processes that act to bridge the gap between genes and these complex behaviours. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   218 citations  
  • Behavior depends on context.Robert W. Smuts - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):33-34.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations