Switch to: References

Citations of:

Darwinism and Human Affairs

Philosophy of Science 48 (4):627-628 (1981)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Etica ed etologia.U. Melotti - 1990 - Global Bioethics 3 (6):35-48.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cross cultural method and the incest taboo.Stephen Beckerman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):263-264.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An evolutionary analysis of rules regulating human inbreeding and marriage.Nancy Wilmsen Thornhill - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):247-261.
    Evolutionary theory predicts that humans should avoid incest because of the negative effects incest has on individual reproduction: production of defective offspring. Selection for the avoidance of close-kin mating has apparently resulted in a psychological mechanism that promotes voluntary incest avoidance. Most human societies are thought to have rules regulating incest. If incest is avoided, why are social rules constructed to regulate it? This target article suggests that incest rules do not exist primarily to regulate close-kin mating but to regulate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Muddled theory and misinterpreted data: Comments on yet another attempt to identify a so-called Westermarck effect and, in the process, to refute Freud.David H. Spain - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):278-279.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mental mechanisms underlying inbreeding rule making.Nancy Wilmsen Thornhill - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):281-293.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intelligence, reproductive success, and social status: A complicated relationship.James D. Weinrich - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):209-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Do we need cultural inertia to explain matrilineal inheritance?Gerald Borgia - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):670-671.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Paternity irrelevance and matrilineal descent.Robin Fox - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):674-675.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Sexual strategies and social-class differences in fitness in modern industrial societies.Hillard Kaplan & Kim Hill - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):198-201.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Further evidence for secular increases in intelligence in Britain, Japan, and the United States.Richard Lynn & Susan Hampson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):203-204.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “eugenic dilemma” revisited.James V. Neel - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):205-205.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Scotch'd the snake, not killed it.Peter T. Saunders & Mae-Wan Ho - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):83-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Familiarity out-breeds.Patrick Bateson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):71-72.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • An alternative explanation for low or zero sib correlations.David T. Lykken - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):31-31.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On nonheritable genetic differences.John Hartung - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):25-25.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Culture and the evolution of learning.H. Ronald Pulliam - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):247-248.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On natural selection and culture.F. T. Cloak - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):238-240.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mate selection: Economics and affection.Kim Wallen - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):37-38.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   59 citations  
  • Another intriguing data bank for use in testing culture-related hypotheses.Walter J. Lonner - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):27-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The sociobiology of human mate preference: On testing evolutionary hypotheses.Nadav Nur - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):28-29.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Characteristics of female desirability: Facultative standards of beauty.Nancy Wilmsen Thornhill - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):35-36.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Diversity: A historical/comparative perspective.Ray H. Bixler - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):15-16.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stability and variation in human evolution.Lionel Tiger - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):115-116.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures.David M. Buss - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):1-14.
    Contemporary mate preferences can provide important clues to human reproductive history. Little is known about which characteristics people value in potential mates. Five predictions were made about sex differences in human mate preferences based on evolutionary conceptions of parental investment, sexual selection, human reproductive capacity, and sexual asymmetries regarding certainty of paternity versus maternity. The predictions centered on how each sex valued earning capacity, ambition— industriousness, youth, physical attractiveness, and chastity. Predictions were tested in data from 37 samples drawn from (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   316 citations  
  • Inbreeding, cousin marriage, and social solidarity.Umberto Melotti - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):112-113.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The maintenance of behavioral diversity in human societies.Christopher Wills - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):638-639.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Group selection: The theory replaces the bogey man.David Sloan Wilson & Elliott Sober - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):639-654.
    In both biology and the human sciences, social groups are sometimes treated as adaptive units whose organization cannot be reduced to individual interactions. This group-level view is opposed by a more individualistic one that treats social organization as a byproduct of self-interest. According to biologists, group-level adaptations can evolve only by a process of natural selection at the group level. Most biologists rejected group selection as an important evolutionary force during the 1960s and 1970s but a positive literature began to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • We are far from understanding sex-related differences in spatial-mathematical abilities despite the theory of sexual selection.Üner Tan - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):264-264.
    I have provided evidence that Geary's model does not explain male dominance in spatial abilities by sexual selection. The current literature concerning the relations of nonverbal IQ to testosterone, hand preference, and right- and left-hand skill, as well as the organizing effects of testosterone on cerebral lateralization during the perinatal period, does not support Geary's arguments.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sex differences and evolutionary by-products.Thomas Wynn, Forrest Tierson & Craig Palmer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):265-266.
    From the perspective of evolutionary theory, we believe it makes more sense to view the sex differences in spatial cognition as being an evolutionary by-product of selection for optimal rates of fetal development. Geary does not convince us that his proposed selective factors operated with “sufficient precision, economy, and efficiency.” Moreover, the archaeological evidence does not support his proposed evolutionary scenario.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mary has more: Sex differences, autism, coherence, and theory of mind.Uta Frith & Francesca Happé - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):253-254.
    We challenge the notion that differences in spatial ability are the best or only explanation for observed sex differences in mathematical word problems. We suggest two ideas from the study of autism: sex differences in theory of mind and in central coherence.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sexual selection and sex differences in mathematical abilities.David C. Geary - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):229-247.
    The principles of sexual selection were used as an organizing framework for interpreting cross-national patterns of sex differences in mathematical abilities. Cross-national studies suggest that there are no sex differences in biologically primary mathematical abilities, that is, for those mathematical abilities that are found in all cultures as well as in nonhuman primates, and show moderate heritability estimates. Sex differences in several biologically secondary mathematical domains are found throughout the industrialized world. In particular, males consistently outperform females in the solving (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Lineage interests and nonreproductive strategies.Erica Hill - 1999 - Human Nature 10 (2):109-134.
    The nonreproductive role of religious women in the European Middle Ages presents the ideal forum for the discussion of elite family strategies within a historical context. I apply the evolutionary concept of kin selection to this group of women in order to explain how a social formation in which religious women failed to reproduce benefited medieval noble lineages. After a brief review of the roles of noble women in the later Middle Ages, I identify two benefits that nonreproductive women provided (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • American legacies and the variable life histories of women and men.Debra S. Judge - 1995 - Human Nature 6 (4):291-323.
    Sex differences in behavior are most interesting when they are the result of inherent differences in the operational rules motivating behavior and not merely a reflection of differing life history experiences. American men and women exhibit a few differences in testamentary patterns of property allocation that appear to be due to inherently different rules of allocation. Even when analyses control for resources and surviving kin configurations, women distribute their property among a greater number of individual beneficiaries than do men. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Behavioral ecology of conservation in traditional societies.Bobbi S. Low - 1996 - Human Nature 7 (4):353-379.
    A common exhortation by conservationists suggests that we can solve ecological problems by returning to the attitudes of traditional societies: reverence for resources, and willingness to assume short-term individual costs for long-term, group-beneficial sustainable management. This paper uses the 186-society Standard Cross-Cultural Sample to examine resource attitudes and practices. Two main findings emerge: (1) resource practices are ecologically driven and do not appear to correlate with attitude (including sacred prohibition) and (2) the low ecological impact of many traditional societies results (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The function of menstrual taboos among the dogon.Beverly I. Strassmann - 1992 - Human Nature 3 (2):89-131.
    Menstrual taboos are nearly ubiquitous and assume parallel forms in geographically distant populations, yet their function has baffled researchers for decades. This paper proposes that menstrual taboos are anticuckoldry tactics. By signaling menstruation, they may advertise female reproductive status to husbands, affines, and other observers. Females may therefore have difficulty in obfuscating the timing of the onset of pregnancy. This may have three consequences: (a) males are better able to assess their probabilities of paternity and to direct their parental investment (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Kinship, sex, and fitness in a Caribbean community.Robert J. Quinlan & Mark V. Flinn - 2005 - Human Nature 16 (1):32-57.
    Patterns of human kinship commonly involve preferential treatment of relatives based on lineal descent (lineages) rather than degree of genetic relatedness (kindreds), presenting a challenge for inclusive fitness theory. Here, we examine effects of lineage and kindred characteristics on reproductive success (RS) and number of grandchildren for 130 men and 124 women in a horticultural community on Dominica. Kindreds had little effect on fitness independently of lineage characteristics. Fitness increased with the number of lineal relatives residing in the community but (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?John Archer - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):249-266.
    I argue that the magnitude and nature of sex differences in aggression, their development, causation, and variability, can be better explained by sexual selection than by the alternative biosocial version of social role theory. Thus, sex differences in physical aggression increase with the degree of risk, occur early in life, peak in young adulthood, and are likely to be mediated by greater male impulsiveness, and greater female fear of physical danger. Male variability in physical aggression is consistent with an alternative (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Science education and moral education.Holmes Rolston - 1988 - Zygon 23 (3):347-355.
    Both science and ethics are embedded in cultural traditions where truths are shared through education; both need competent critics educated within such traditions. Education in both ought to be directed although moral education demands levels of responsible agency that science education does not. Evolutionary science often carries an implicit or explicit understanding of who and what humans are, one which may not be coherent with the implicit or explicit human self‐understanding in moral education. The latter in turn may not be (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Wat is menselijk? Wat is wenselijk?Pouwel Slurink - 2006 - Krisis 7 (1):26-41.
    Relatively short Dutch introduction to an evolutionary approach to morality. A synthesis is given of various models of moral evolution. Some remarks are made on a way to look at the evolution of a compatibilistic 'free will' and a model is given of a way in which the 'good' can be understood as the results of shared interests (which, of course, gives an incomplete model, but at the same time throws a lot of light on the way in which we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Runaway Social Selection for Displays of Partner Value and Altruism.Randolph M. Nesse - 2007 - Biological Theory 2 (2):143-155.
    Runaway social selection resulting from partner choice may have shaped aspects of human cooperation and complex sociality that are otherwise hard to account for. Social selection is the subtype of natural selection that results from the social behaviors of other individuals. Competition to be chosen as a social partner can, like competition to be chosen as a mate, result in runaway selection that shapes extreme traits. People prefer partners who display valuable resources and bestow them selectively on close partners. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • The intertwined roles of genes and culture in human evolution.William Irons - 2009 - Zygon 44 (2):347-354.
    This essay critiques dual-inheritance theory as presented in Peter Richerson and Robert Boyd's book Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution (2005). The theory states that culture became prominent in human evolution because it allowed relatively rapid adaptation to changing environments by means of imitation. Imitating the behavior of other members of one's community produces adaptive behaviors more readily than either genetic evolution or individual learning. Imitation follows a number of patterns: imitating high-status individuals, imitating the most common (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Biological Justification of Ethics: A Best-Case Scenario.Alexander Rosenberg - 1990 - Social Philosophy and Policy 8 (1):86.
    Social and behavioral scientists - that is, students of human nature - nowadays hardly ever use the term ‘human nature’. This reticence reflects both a becoming modesty about the aims of their disciplines and a healthy skepticism about whether there is any one thing really worthy of the label ‘human nature’.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Obligation and the new naturalism.Roger D. Masters - 1989 - Biology and Philosophy 4 (1):17-32.
    Although it has become increasingly evident that an adequate theory of obligation must rest on evolutionary biology and human ethology, attempts toward this end need to explore the full range of personal, cultural, and political obligations observed in our species. The new naturalism reveals the complexity of social behavior and the defects of reductionist models that oversimplify the foundations of human duties and rights. Ultimately, this approach suggest a return to the Aristotelian concept of natural justice.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • (1 other version)Critical notice.Kim Sterelny - 1988 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 66 (4):538 – 555.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Psychoanalytic theory and incest avoidance rules.Robert A. Paul - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):276-277.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rules regulating inbreeding, cultural variability and the great heuristic problem of evolutionary anthropology.Eckart Voland - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):279-280.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A little more mortar for a firm foundation.Laura Betzig - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):264-264.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social and reproductive success: Useful data but rethink the theory.William Irons - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):197-198.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Passion for sexual pleasure, the measurement of selection, and prospects for eugenics.Carl Jay Bajema - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):187-188.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Wealth, polygyny, and reproductive success.Richard Dawkins - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):190-191.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations