Switch to: References

Citations of:

Natural Language Understanding

Benjamin Cummings (1995)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Situated semantics.Varol Akman - 2008 - In Murat Aydede & P. Robbins (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 401-418.
    Situated semantics can be regarded as an attempt at placing situational context (context of situation) at the center of all discussions of meaning. Situation theory is a theory of information content that takes context very seriously. Individuals, properties, relations, and spatiotemporal locations are basic constructs of situation theory. Individuals are conceived as invariants; having properties and standing in relations, they tend to persist in time and space. An anchoring function binds the location parameters to appropriate objects present in the grounding (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Computational and biological constraints in the psychology of reasoning.Mike Oaksford & Mike Malloch - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):468-469.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Psychological implications of the synchronicity hypothesis.Stellan Ohlsson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):469-469.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What we know and the LTKB.Stanley Munsat - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):466-467.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Time phases, pointers, rules and embedding.John A. Barnden - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):451-452.
    This paper is a commentary on the target article by Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde [S&A]: “From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony” in same issue of the journal, pp.417–451. -/- It puts S&A's temporal-synchrony binding method in a broader context, comments on notions of pointing and other ways of associating information - in both computers and connectionist systems - and mentions types of reasoning that are a challenge to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From symbols to neurons: Are we there yet?Garrison W. Cottrell - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):454-454.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Must we solve the binding problem in neural hardware?James W. Garson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):459-460.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Relational Order and Onto-Thematic Roles.Francesco Orilia - 2011 - Metaphysica 12 (1):1-18.
    States of affairs involving a non-symmetric relation such as loving are said to have a relational order, something that distinguishes, for instance, Romeo’s loving Juliet from Juliet’s loving Romeo. Relational order can be properly understood by appealing to o-roles, i.e., ontological counterparts of what linguists call thematic roles, e.g., agent, patient, instrument, and the like. This move allows us to meet the appropriate desiderata for a theory of relational order. In contrast, the main theories that try to do without o-roles, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Underspecified semantics.Reinhard Muskens - 2000 - In Klaus von Heusinger & Urs Egli (eds.), Reference and Anaphoric Relations. Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 311--338.
    Ambiguities in natural language can multiply so fast that no person or machine can be expected to process a text of even moderate length by enumerating all possible disambiguations. A sentence containing $n$ scope bearing elements which are freely permutable will have $n!$ readings, if there are no other, say lexical or syntactic, sources of ambiguity. A series of $m$ such sentences would lead to $(n!)^m$ possibilities. All in all the growth of possibilities will be so fast that generating readings (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Steps toward formalizing context.Varol Akman & Mehmet Surav - 1996 - AI Magazine 17 (3):55-72.
    The importance of contextual reasoning is emphasized by various researchers in AI. (A partial list includes John McCarthy and his group, R. V. Guha, Yoav Shoham, Giuseppe Attardi and Maria Simi, and Fausto Giunchiglia and his group.) Here, we survey the problem of formalizing context and explore what is needed for an acceptable account of this abstract notion.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Contesti in intelligenza artificiale: una fugace rassegna (Context in artificial intelligence: a fleeting overview).Varol Akman - 2002 - In Carlo Penco (ed.), La svolta contestuale. New York: McGraw-Hill.
    The notion of context arises in assorted areas of artificial intelligence (AI), including knowledge representation, natural language processing, intelligent information retrieval, etc. Although the term ‘context’ is frequently employed in descriptions, explanations, and analyses of computer programs in these areas, its meaning is frequently left to the reader’s understanding. -/- My aim in this paper is to offer a swift review of context in AI. I will first identify the role of context in various fields of AI. I will then (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Temporal adverbials, tenses and the perfect.Frank Vlach - 1993 - Linguistics and Philosophy 16 (3):231 - 283.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Plausible reasoning and the resolution of quantifier scope ambiguities.Walid S. Saba & Jean-Pierre Corriveau - 2001 - Studia Logica 67 (2):271-289.
    Despite overwhelming evidence suggesting that quantifier scope is a phenomenon that must be treated at the pragmatic level, most computational treatments of scope ambiguities have thus far been a collection of syntactically motivated preference rules. This might be in part due to the prevailing wisdom that a commonsense inferencing strategy would require the storage of and reasoning with a vast amount of background knowledge. In this paper we hope to demonstrate that the challenge in developing a commonsense inferencing strategy is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A grammar systems approach to natural language grammar.M. Dolores Jiménez López - 2006 - Linguistics and Philosophy 29 (4):419 - 454.
    Taking as its starting point significant similarities between a formal language model—Grammar Systems—and a grammatical theory—Autolexical Syntax—in this paper we suggest the application of the former to the topic of the latter. To show the applicability of Grammar Systems Theory to grammatical description, we introduce a formal-language-theoretic framework for the architecture of natural language grammar: Linguistic Grammar Systems. We prove the adequacy of this model by highlighting its features (modularity, parallelism, interaction) and by showing the similarity between this framework and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inference and computational semantics.Patrick Blackburn & Michael Kohlhase - 2004 - Journal of Logic, Language and Information 13 (2):117-120.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Consciousness and conceptual learning in a socially situated agent.Myles Bogner, Uma Ramamurthy & Stan Franklin - 2000 - In Kerstin Dauthenhahn (ed.), Human Cognition and Social Agent Technology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 113--135.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables, and dynamic binding using temporal synchrony.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):417-51.
    Human agents draw a variety of inferences effortlessly, spontaneously, and with remarkable efficiency – as though these inferences were a reflexive response of their cognitive apparatus. Furthermore, these inferences are drawn with reference to a large body of background knowledge. This remarkable human ability seems paradoxical given the complexity of reasoning reported by researchers in artificial intelligence. It also poses a challenge for cognitive science and computational neuroscience: How can a system of simple and slow neuronlike elements represent a large (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   122 citations  
  • Rethinking context as a social construct.Varol Akman - 2000 - Journal of Pragmatics 32 (6):743-759.
    This paper argues that in addition to the familiar approach using formal contexts, there is now a need in artificial intelligence to study contexts as social constructs. As a successful example of the latter approach, I draw attention to 'interpretation' (in the sense of literary theory), viz. the reconstruction of the intended meaning of a literary text that takes into account the context in which the author assumed the reader would place the text. An important contribution here comes from Wendell (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Views from a cognitive scientist: cognitive representations underlying discourse are sometimes social.Arthur C. Graesser - 2006 - Discourse Studies 8 (1):59-66.
    Most areas of the cognitive and social sciences assume that knowledge representations are constructed and used during communication and that much of its content is social. Those of us who build computer models of comprehension and conversation are forced to be explicit about the nature of these knowledge representations and affiliated processes. There are some conditions when knowledge is not sufficiently social, and other conditions when knowledge is overly grounded in social mechanisms. The argument is advanced that constraints, coherence, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Communication and content.Prashant Parikh - 2019 - Berlin, Germany: Language Science Press.
    Communication and content presents a comprehensive and foundational account of meaning based on new versions of situation theory and game theory. The literal and implied meanings of an utterance are derived from first principles assuming little more than the partial rationality of interacting agents. New analyses of a number of diverse phenomena – a wide notion of ambiguity and content encompassing phonetics, syntax, semantics, pragmatics, and beyond, vagueness, convention and conventional meaning, indeterminacy, universality, the role of truth in communication, semantic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Positions, Ordering Relations and O‐Roles.Francesco Orilia - 2014 - Dialectica 68 (2):283-303.
    This paper first discusses how Russell and Hochberg have addressed some phenomena of relatedness, notably relational order, in a similarly ‘positionalist’ way, yet by appealing to different sorts of formal relations: “positions” in Russell's case and “ordering relations” in Hochberg's. After pointing out some shortcomings of both approaches, the paper then proposes an alternative view based on ‘o-roles’, which are, roughly speaking, ontological counterparts of the thematic roles postulated in linguistics. It is argued that o-roles are sort of middle-of-the-road entities (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • A computational model of argumentation in agreement negotiation processes.Mare Koit & Haldur Õim - 2015 - Argument and Computation 6 (2):101-129.
    The paper describes a computational model that we have implemented in an experimental dialogue system. Communication in a natural language between two participants A and B is considered, where A has a communicative goal that his/her partner B will make a decision to perform an action D. A argues the usefulness, pleasantness, etc. of D, in order to guide B's reasoning in a desirable direction. A computational model of argumentation is developed, which includes reasoning. Our model is based on the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Selectional constraints: an information-theoretic model and its computational realization.Philip Resnik - 1996 - Cognition 61 (1-2):127-159.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • A step toward modeling reflexive reasoning.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):477-494.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Ethereal oscillations.Malcolm P. Young - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):476-477.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Should first-order logic be neurally plausible?David S. Touretzky & Scott E. Fahlman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):474-475.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do simple associations lead to systematic reasoning?Steven Sloman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-472.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Phase logic is biologically relevant logic.Gary W. Strong - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):472-473.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Temporal synchrony and the speed of visual processing.Simon J. Thorpe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):473-474.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Making reasoning more reasonable: Event-coherence and assemblies.Günther Palm - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):470-470.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Useful ideas for exploiting time to engineer representations.Richard Rohwer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-471.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Synchronization and cognitive carpentry: From systematic structuring to simple reasoning. E. Koerner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):465-466.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reflections on reflexive reasoning.David L. Martin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):466-466.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rule acquisition and variable binding: Two sides of the same coin.P. J. Hampson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):462-462.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Not all reflexive reasoning is deductive.Graeme Hirst & Dekai Wu - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):462-463.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the artificial intelligence paradox.Steffen Hölldobler - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):463-464.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Distributing structure over time.John E. Hummel & Keith J. Holyoak - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):464-464.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Self-organizing neural models of categorization, inference and synchrony.Stephen Grossberg - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):460-461.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Competing, or perhaps complementary, approaches to the dynamic-binding problem, with similar capacity limitations.Graeme S. Halford - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):461-462.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Dynamic bindings by real neurons: Arguments from physiology, neural network models and information theory.Reinhard Eckhorn - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):457-458.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward a unified behavioral and brain science.Jerome A. Feldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):458-458.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Deconstruction of neural data yields biologically implausible periodic oscillations.Walter J. Freeman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):458-459.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Reasoning, learning and neuropsychological plausibility.Joachim Diederich - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):455-456.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionism and syntactic binding of concepts.Georg Dorffner - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):456-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Could static binding suffice?Paul R. Cooper - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):453-454.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Making a middling mousetrap.Michael R. W. Dawson & Istvan Berkeley - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):454-455.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Plausible inference and implicit representation.Malcolm I. Bauer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):452-453.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Representation and Processing of Coreference in Discourse.Peter C. Gordon & Randall Hendrick - 1998 - Cognitive Science 22 (4):389-424.
    A model is presented that addresses both the distribution and comprehension of different forms of referring expressions in language. This model is expressed in a formalism (Kamp & Reyle, 1993) that uses interpretive rules to map syntactic representations onto representations of discourse. Basic interpretive rules are developed for names, pronouns, definite descriptions, and quantified descriptions. These rules are triggered by syntactic input and interact dynamically with representations of discourse to establish reference and coreference. This interaction determines the ease with which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • A Bayesian‐Network Approach to Lexical Disambiguation.Leila M. R. Eizirik, Valmir C. Barbosa & Sueli B. T. Mendes - 1993 - Cognitive Science 17 (2):257-283.
    Lexical ambiguity can be syntactic if it involves more than one grammatical category for a single word, or semantic if more than one meaning can be associated with a word. In this article we discuss the application of a Bayesian‐network model in the resolution of lexical ambiguities of both types. The network we propose comprises a parsing subnetwork, which can be constructed automatically for any context‐free grammar, and a subnetwork for semantic analysis, which, in the spirit of Fillmore's (1968) case (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark