Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Sharpening the focus on functions of the hippocampus.Daniel P. Kimble - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):504-505.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Putting anxiety in its place?Daniel P. Kimble - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):489-489.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Eysenck's model of neurotigenesis.H. D. Kimmel - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):171-172.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dopamine and mental illness: Phenomenological and anatomical considerations.Ann E. Kelley - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):219-220.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Parting's sweet sorrow: A pain pathway for the social sentiments?Leonard D. Katz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):435-436.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Where have all the peptides gone?Peter W. Kalivas - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):218-219.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A two-tiered theory of emotions: Affect and feeling.Julian Jaynes - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):434-435.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • The prefrontal cortex — accumbens circuit: Who's in charge?George E. Jaskiw & Daniel R. Weinberger - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):217-218.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • On the hippocampus, time, and interference.Leonard E. Jarrard - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):503-504.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • The evolution of hesitation, doubt, and map-making.D. T. D. James - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):488-489.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does our behavioral methodology conceal the deficit caused by hippocampal damage?David T. D. James - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):502-503.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From stimulus-bound emotive command systems to drive-free emotions.C. E. Izard - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):433-434.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Integrating the literature on anxiety, memory, and the hippocampus.Susan D. Iversen - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):487-488.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Information synthesis in cortical areas as an important link in brain mechanisms of mind.Alexei M. Ivanitsky - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):686-687.
    To explore the mechanism of sensation correlations between EP component amplitude and signal detection indices were studied. The time of sensation coincided with the peak latency of those EP components that showed a correlation with both indices. The components presumably reflected information synthesis in projection cortical neurons. A mechanism providing the synthesis process is proposed.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mind – your head!R. P. Ingvaldsen & H. T. A. Whiting - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):685-686.
    Gray takes an information-processing paradigm as his departure point, invoking a comparator as part of the system. He concludes that consciousness is to be found “in” the comparator but is unable to point to how the comparison takes place. Thus, the comparator turns out not to be an entity arising out of brain research per se, but out of the logic of the paradigm. In this way, Gray both reinvents dualism and remains trapped in the language game of his own (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Perspective, reflection, transparent explanation, and other minds.S. L. Hurley - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):684-685.
    Perspective and reflection have each been considered in some way basic to phenomenal consciousness. Each has possible evolutionary value, though neither seems sufficient for consciousness. Consider an account of consciousness in terms of the combination of perspective and reflection, its relationship to the problem of other minds, and its capacity to inherit evolutionary explanation from its components.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Temporal discontiguity: Alternative to, or component of, existing theories of hippocampal function?Donna J. Hughey - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):501-502.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • On the sufficiency of a Pavlovian conditioning model for coping with the complexities of neurosis.Arne Öhman & Holger Ursin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):179-180.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Psychopathology and the discontinuity of conscious experience.David R. Hemsley - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):683-684.
    It is accepted that “primary awareness” may emerge from the integration of two classes of information. It is unclear, however, why this cannot take place within the comparator rather than in conjunction with feedback to the perceptual systems. The model has plausibility in relation to the continuity of conscious experience in the normal waking state and may be extended to encompass certain aspects of the “sense of self” which are frequently disrupted in psychotic patients.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Panksepp's psychobiological theory of emotions: Some substantiation.Robert G. Heath - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):432-433.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effects of percentage of reinforcement and number of reinforcements in S+ on discrimination learning in the runway.Steven J. Haggbloom & E. J. Capaldi - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (4):283-286.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effects of magnitude of reward and intensity of intermittent punishment on resistance to extinction.Nabil F. Haddad & Roger L. Mellgren - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 7 (5):449-451.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neural Dynamics of Autistic Repetitive Behaviors and Fragile X Syndrome: Basal Ganglia Movement Gating and mGluR-Modulated Adaptively Timed Learning.Stephen Grossberg & Devika Kishnan - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The contents of consciousness: A neuropsychological conjecture.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):659-76.
    Drawing on previous models of anxiety, intermediate memory, the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, and goal-directed behaviour, a neuropsychological hypothesis is proposed for the generation of the contents of consciousness. It is suggested that these correspond to the outputs of a comparator that, on a moment-by-moment basis, compares the current state of the organism's perceptual world with a predicted state. An outline is given of the information-processing functions of the comparator system and of the neural systems which mediate them. The hypothesis (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):469-484.
    A model of the neuropsychology of anxiety is proposed. The model is based in the first instance upon an analysis of the behavioural effects of the antianxiety drugs in animals. From such psychopharmacologi-cal experiments the concept of a “behavioural inhibition system” has been developed. This system responds to novel stimuli or to those associated with punishment or nonreward by inhibiting ongoing behaviour and increasing arousal and attention to the environment. It is activity in the BIS that constitutes anxiety and that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   557 citations  
  • On the classification of the emotions.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):431-432.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • On mapping anxiety.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):506-534.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Memory buffer and comparator can share the same circuitry.J. A. Gray - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (3):501-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Is there any need for conditioning in Eysenck's conditioning model of neurosis?Jeffrey A. Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):169-171.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Don't leave the “psych” out of neuropsychology.Jeffrey A. Gray & Ilan Baruch - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):215-217.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Consciousness and its (dis)contents.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):703-722.
    The first claim in the target article was that there is as yet no transparent, causal account of the relations between consciousness and brain-and-behaviour. That claim remains firm. The second claim was that the contents of consciousness consist, psychologically, of the outputs of a comparator system; the third consisted of a description of the brain mechanisms proposed to instantiate the comparator. In order to defend these claims against criticism, it has been necessary to clarify the distinction between consciousness-as-such and the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Brain Systems that Mediate both Emotion and Cognition.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1990 - Cognition and Emotion 4 (3):269-288.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • An electrophysiologist's eye view of the basal ganglia.Anthony A. Grace - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Antianxiety and opiates.Mark S. Gold & Corinne Frantz Fox - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):486-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can phenomenology contribute to brain science?Gordon G. Globus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):430-431.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The neuropathology of schizophrenia, mania, and depression: Diseases of cognitive initiation and switching?Eliot L. Gardner - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):213-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Homunculus in the subiculum.Michael Gabriel - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):485-486.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rationalist versus empirical approaches to observing and conditioned reinforcement: The preference-for-signaled-shock.J. J. Furedy & G. B. Biederman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):367-367.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Consciousness is for other people.Chris Frith - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):682-683.
    Gray has expanded his account of schizophrenia to explain consciousness as well. His theory explains neither phenomenon adequately because he treats individual minds in isolation. The primary function of consciousness is to permit high level interactions with other conscious beings. The key symptoms of schizophrenia reflect a failure of this mechanism.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • On seeking the mythical fountain of consciousness.Jeffrey Foss - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):682-682.
    Because consciousness has an organizational, or functional, center, Gray supposes that there must be a corresponding physical center in the brain. He proposes further that since this center generates consciousness, ablating it would eliminate consciousness, while leaving behavior intact. But the center of consciousness is simply the product of the functional linkages among sensory input, memory, inner speech, and so on, and behavior.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Emotions are objective events.Elzbieta Fonberg - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):429-430.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural circuit models of psychopathology: Dancing on the precipice of neuromythology?H. C. Fibiger - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):212-213.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The ghost in the machine: What if the midbrain output is excitatory?James H. Fallon - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):210-212.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The conditioning theory of neurosis: criticisms considered.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):188-199.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The conditioning model of neurosis.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):155-166.
    The long-term persistence of neurotic symptoms, such as anxiety, poses difficult problems for any psychological theory. An attempt is made to revive the Watson-Mowrer conditioning theory and to avoid the many criticisms directed against it in the past. It is suggested that recent research has produced changes in learning theory that can be used to render this possible. In the first place, the doctrine of equipotentiality has been shown to be wrong, and some such concept as Seligman's “preparedness” is required, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   96 citations  
  • Context and consciousness.Colin G. Ellard - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):681-682.
    The commentary argues that we cannot be sure that human consciousness has survival value and that in order to understand the origins and, perhaps, the function of consciousness, we should examine the behavioural and neural precursors to consciousness in nonhumans. An example is given of research on the role of context in decisions regarding fleeing from probable predators in the Mongolian gerbil.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Consciousness, memory, and the hippocampal system: What kind of connections can we make?Howard Eichenbaum & Neal J. Cohen - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):680-681.
    Gray's account is remarkable in its depth and scope but too little attention is paid to poor correspondences with the literature on hippocampal/subicular damage, the theta rhythm, and novelty detection. An alternative account, focusing on hippocampal involvement in organizing memories in a way that makes them accessible to conscious recollection but not in access to consciousness per se, avoids each of these limitations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Gantt and Eysenck conditioning models for neurosis.Roscoe A. Dykman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-169.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On preferences for unsignaled shocks and for unpredictable rewards.James A. Dinsmoor - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):368-370.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Introspection as the Rosetta stone: Millstone or fifth wheel?Ronald de Sousa - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):428-429.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark