Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Sensorimotor learning in structures “upstream” from the cerebellum.Paul van Donkelaar - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):477-478.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Equifinality and phase-resetting: The role of control parameter manipulations.R. E. A. van Emmerik & R. C. Wagenaar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):783-784.
    It is argued that the equilibrium point model can lead to new insights regarding transition and stability processes in movement coordination. The role of movement control parameters on equifinality and phase-resetting is discussed; not only control but also external control parameters can affect the global dynamical regime.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Limitations of PET and lesion studies in defining the role of the human cerebellum in motor learning.D. Timmann & H. C. Diener - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):477-477.
    PET studies using classical conditioning paradigms are reported. It is emphasized that PET studies show and not in learning paradigms. The importance of dissociating motor performance and learning deficits in human lesions studies is demonstrated in two exemplary studies. The different role of the cerebellum in adaptation of postural reflexes and learning of complex voluntary arm movements is discussed, [THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motor learning and synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum.Richard F. Thompson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):475-477.
    For reasons I have never understood, some students of the cerebellum have been unwilling to accept the now overwhelming evidence that the cerebellum exhibits lasting synaptic plasticity and plays an essential role in some forms of learning and memory. With a few exceptions (e.g., target article by SIMPSON et al.) this is no longer the case, as is clear in the excellent target articles on cerebellar LTD and the excellent target review by HOUK et al. [CRÉPEL et al.; HOUR et (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Origins of origins of motor control.Esther Thelen - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):780-783.
    Examination of infant spontaneous and goal-directed arm movements supports Feldman and Levin's hypothesis of a functional hierarchy. Early infant movements are dominated by biomechanical and dynamic factors without external frames of reference. Development involves not only learning to generate these frames of reference, but also protecting the higher-level goal of the movement from internal and external perturbations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Q: Is the cerebellum an adaptive combiner of motor and mental/motor activities? A: Yes, maybe, certainly not, who can say?W. Thomas Thach - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):501-528.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • We know a lot about the cerebellum, but do we know what motor learning is?Stephan P. Swinnen, Charles B. Walter & Natalia Dounskaia - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):474-475.
    In the behavioral literature on human movement, a distinction is made between the learning of parameters and the learning of new movement forms or topologies. Whereas the target articles by Thach, Smith, and Houk et al. provide evidence for cerebellar involvement in parametrization learning and adaptation, the evidence in favor of its involvement in the generation of new movement patterns is less straightforward. A case is made for focusing more attention on the latter issue in the future. This would directly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How to link the specificity of cerebellar anatomy to motor learning?Fahad Sultan, Detlef Heck & Harold Bekkering - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):474.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Control parameters, equilibria, and coordination dynamics.Dagmar Sternad & M. T. Turvey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):780-780.
    Important similarities exist between the dynamical concepts implicit in Feldman & Levin's extended λ model and those basic to a dynamical systems approach. We argue that careful application of the key concepts of control and order parameters, equilibria, and stability, can relate known facts of neuromuscular processes to the observables of functional, task-specific behavior.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resilient cerebellar theory complies with stiff opposition.Allan M. Smith - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):499-501.
    In response to several requests from commentators, an unambiguous definition of time-varying joint stiffness is provided. However, since a variety of different operations can be used to measure stiffness, a problem for quantification admittedly still exists. Several commentaries pointed out the advantage of controlling joint stiffness in optimizing the speed-accuracy trade-off known as Fittss law. The deficit in rapid reciprocal movements and the impact on joint stiffness inhibition caused by cerebellar lesions is clarified here, as the target article was apparently (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Two joints are more than twice one joint.Jeroen B. J. Smeets - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):779-780.
    An alternative multi-joint extension to the lambda model is proposed. According to this extension, the activity of a muscle depends not only on the difference between lambda and length of that muscle, but also on the difference between lambda and length of other muscles. This 2-D extension can describe more neurophysiological experiments than the extension proposed in the target article.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • More on climbing fiber signals and their consequence(s).J. I. Simpson, D. R. W. Wylie & C. I. De Zeeuw - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):496-498.
    Several themes can be identified in the commentaries. The first is that the climbing fibers may have more than one function; the second is that the climbing fibers provide sensory rather than motor signals. We accept the possibility that climbing fibers may have more than one function consequence(s)’ in the title. Until we know more about the function of the inhibitory input to the inferior olive from the cerebellar nuclei, which are motor structures, we have to keep open the possibility (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Can the λ model be used to interpret the activity of single neurons?Stephen H. Scott - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):778-779.
    Whereas the λ model provides a useful technique to describe complex movements, the focus on control variables in this model limits its potential for interpreting the activity and function of many cells in motor areas of the CNS.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dysmetria of thought: Correlations and conundrums in the relationship between the cerebellum, learning, and cognitive processing.Jeremy D. Schmahmann - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):472-473.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebellar rhythms: Exploring another metaphor.Patrick D. Roberts, Gin McCollum & Jan E. Holly - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):471-472.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Position is everything?Karl H. Pribram - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):776-778.
    Neurophysiological evidence consonant with F&L's lambda model is reviewed and results of additional experiments are presented. The evidence shows that there are neurons in the motor cortex that respond to selective band widths of passive sinusoidal movements; the additional data show how, with movement, directionally sensitive population vectors can be shown to emerge from the data.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebellar theory out of control.Michael G. Paulin - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):470-471.
    The views of Houk et al., Smith, and Thach on the role of cerebellum in movement control differ substantially, but all three are flawed by the false reasoning that because information passes from the cerebellum to movements the cerebellum must be a movement controller, or a part of one. The divergent and less than compelling ideas expressed by these leading cerebellar theorists epitomize the fruitlessness of this paradigm, and signal the need for a change. [HOUK et al.; SMITH; THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The λ model: Can it walk?Aftab E. Patla - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):775-776.
    Generation of swing phase limb trajectory over obstacles during locomotion should be a reasonable test for the λ model proposed by Feldman and Levin. The observed features such as lack of simple amplitude scaling of endpoint (toe) trajectories for different obstacle heights, complex shaped toe velocity profiles, and exploitation of passive intersegmental dynamics to control limb elevation cannot be adequately explained by the λ model.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Let us accept a “controlled trade-off” model of motor control.Lloyd D. Partridge - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):773-775.
    The trade-off between force and length of muscle as adjusted by neural signals is a critical fact in the dynamics of motor control. Whether we call it “length-tension effect,” “feedback-like,” “invariant condition,” or “spring-like” is unimportant. We must not let semantics or details of representation obscure the basic physics of effects introduced by this trade-off in muscle.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spatial frames for motor control would be commensurate with spatial frames for vision and proprioception, but what about control of energy flows?Christopher C. Pagano & Geoffrey P. Bingham - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):773-773.
    The model identifies a spatial coordinate frame within which the sensorimotor apparatus produces movement. Its spatial nature simplifies its coupling with spatial reference frames used concurrently by vision and proprioception. While the positional reference frame addresses the performance of spatial tasks, it seems to have little to say about movements involving energy expenditure as the principle component of the task.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Command invariants and the frame of reference for human movement.David J. Ostry, Rafael Laboissière & Paul L. Gribble - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):770-772.
    We describe a solution to the redundancy problem related to that proposed in Feldman & Levin's target article. We suggest that the system may use a fixed mapping between commands organized at the level of degrees of freedom and commands to individual muscles. This proposal eliminates the need to maintain an explicit representation of musculoskeletalgeometry in planning movements.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cerebellum and cerebral cortex: Contrasting and converging contributions to spatial navigation and memory.Shane M. O'Mara - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):469-470.
    Thach's target article presents a remarkable overview and integration of animal and human studies on the functions of the cerebellum and makes clear theoretical predictions for both the normal operation of the cerebellum and for the effects of cerebellar lesions in the mature human. Commentary is provided on three areas, namely, spatial navigation, implicit learning, and cerebellar agenesis to elicit further development of the themes already present in Thach's paper, [THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Nitric oxide is involved in cerebellar long-term depression.Daisuke Okada - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):468-469.
    The involvement of nitric oxide in cerebellar long-term depression is supported by the observation that nitric oxide is released by climbing fiber stimulation and by pharmacological tool usage. Two forms of long-term depression should be distinguished by their physiological relevance. [CRÉPEL et al.; LINDEN; VINCENT].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Interneurons as backseat drivers and the elusive control variable.T. Richard Nichols - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):772-773.
    It is proposed here that the spinal network of proprioceptive feedback from length and force receptors constitutes the mechanism underlying the coordination of activation thresholds for muscles acting about the same and neighboring joints. For the most part, these circuits come between motoneurons and supraspinal signals, invalidating the idea that the activation thresholds constitute control variables for the motor system.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Kinematic invariances and body schema.Pietro Morasso & Vittorio Sanguineti - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):769-770.
    Generalizing the notion that muscles are positional frames of reference, a high-dimensional muscle space is defined for multi-muscle systems with an embedded low-dimensional motor manifold of functional articulators. A central representation of such a manifold is proposed as computational body schema. The example of the jaw-tongue system is presented, discussing the relation of functional articulators with kinematic invariances and control problems.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Further evidence for the involvement of nitric oxide in trans-ACPD-induced suppression of AMPA responses in cultured chick Purkinje neurons.Junko Mori-Okamoto & Koichi Okamoto - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):467-468.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensory prediction as a role for the cerebellum.R. C. Miall, M. Malkmus & E. M. Robertson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):466-467.
    We suggest that the cerebellum generates sensory or estimates based on outgoing motor commands and sensory feedback. Thus, it is not a motor pattern generator (HOUK et al.) but a predictive system which is intimately involved in motor behavior. This theory may explain the sensitivity of the climbing fibers to both unexpected external events and motor errors (SIMPSON et al.), and we speculate that unusual biophysical properties of the inferior olive might allow the cerebellum to develop multiple asynchronous sensory estimates, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can the λ model benefit from understanding human adaptation in weightlessness(and vice versa)?P. Vernon McDonald - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):768-768.
    Parameters of the lambda model seem tightly linked to certain characteristics of human performance influenced by weightlessness. This commentary suggests that there is a valuable opportunity to probe the lambda model using the changed environment experienced during space flight. The likely benefits are a better model and a better understanding ofthe consequences of weightlessness for human performance.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark