- Bilinguals’ Sensitivity to Grammatical Gender Cues in Russian: The Role of Cumulative Input, Proficiency, and Dominance.Natalia Mitrofanova, Yulia Rodina, Olga Urek & Marit Westergaard - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.details
|
|
Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon.William Marslen-Wilson, Lorraine K. Tyler, Rachelle Waksler & Lianne Older - 1994 - Psychological Review 101 (1):3-33.details
|
|
On computational theories and multilevel, multitask models of cognition: The case of word recognition.Arthur M. Jacobs - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):670-672.details
|
|
Toward a theory of human memory: Data structures and access processes.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):655-667.details
|
|
Rules Versus Statistics: Insights From a Highly Inflected Language.Jelena Mirković, Mark S. Seidenberg & Marc F. Joanisse - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (4):638-681.details
|
|
Priming is not all bias: Commentary on Ratcliff and McKoon (1997).Jeffrey S. Bowers - 1999 - Psychological Review 106 (3):582-596.details
|
|
Investigating the flow of information during speaking: the impact of morpho-phonological, associative, and categorical picture distractors on picture naming.Jens Bölte, Andrea Böhl, Christian Dobel & Pienie Zwitserlood - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.details
|
|
Morphological Processing as We Know It: An Analytical Review of Morphological Effects in Visual Word Identification.Simona Amenta & Davide Crepaldi - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.details
|
|
The representation of Hebrew words: Evidence from the obligatory contour principle.Iris Berent & Joseph Shimron - 1997 - Cognition 64 (1):39-72.details
|
|
What are the “goals” of the human memory system?David J. Murray - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):676-677.details
|
|
Does a computational theory of human memory need intelligence?Sachiko Kinoshita - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):673-674.details
|
|
Lexical storage and regular processes.Geert Booij - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1016-1016.details
|
|
Beyond the Tower of Babel in human memory research: The validity and utility of specification.Michael S. Humphreys, Janet Wiles & Simon Dennis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):682-692.details
|
|
Brain damage and cognitive dysfunction.Marlene Oscar-Berman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):678-679.details
|
|
Marr versus Marr: On the notion of levels.Frank van der Velde, Gezinus Wolters & A. H. C. van der Heijden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):681-682.details
|
|
Can we really dissociate the computational and algorithm-level theories of human memory?Guy Tiberghien - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):680-681.details
|
|
Overtensing and the effect of regularity.Joseph Paul Stemberger - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (6):737-766.details
|
|
Progress within the bounds of memory.Steven A. Sloman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):679-680.details
|
|
Neglected Factors Bearing on Reaction Time in Language Production.Tobias Scheer & Fabien Mathy - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (10):13050.details
|
|
Evidence from neglect dyslexia for morphological decomposition at the early stages of orthographic-visual analysis.Julia Reznick & Naama Friedmann - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.details
|
|
Is the representation meaningful? A measurement theoretic view.In Jae Myung - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):677-678.details
|
|
Caught in a bind: Context information and episodic memory.Kevin Murnane - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):675-676.details
|
|
Syntactic category constrains lexical competition in speaking.Shota Momma, Julia Buffinton, L. Robert Slevc & Colin Phillips - 2020 - Cognition 197:104183.details
|
|
Task-specification language, or theory of human memory?Richard L. Lewis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):674-675.details
|
|
Memory and social cognition.Yoshihisa Kashima - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):672-673.details
|
|
The cognitive RISC machine needs complexity.Richard A. Heath - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):669-670.details
|
|
Why do we need a computational theory of laboratory tasks?Robert L. Greene - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-669.details
|
|
Strong and weak formal specifications.Richard M. Golden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):668-668.details
|
|
Orthographic units in the absence of visual processing: Evidence from sublexical structure in braille.Simon Fischer-Baum & Robert Englebretson - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):161-174.details
|
|
Decomposability and mental representation of French verbs.Gustavo L. Estivalet & Fanny E. Meunier - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.details
|
|
Inorganic memory.Thomas L. Clarke - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):667-667.details
|
|
Identity and similarity factors in repetition blindness: implications for lexical processing.Doriana Chialant & Alfonso Caramazza - 1997 - Cognition 63 (1):79-119.details
|
|
Morpheme-based reading aloud: Evidence from dyslexic and skilled Italian readers.Cristina Burani, Stefania Marcolini, Maria De Luca & Pierluigi Zoccolotti - 2008 - Cognition 108 (1):243-262.details
|
|
Manipulations of word frequency reveal differences in the processing of morphologically complex and simple words in German.Maria Bronk, Pienie Zwitserlood & Jens Bölte - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.details
|
|
Affixal Homonymy triggers full-form storage, even with inflected words, even in a morphologically rich language.R. Bertram - 2000 - Cognition 74 (2):B13-B25.details
|
|
Is “Hit and Run” a Single Word? The Processing of Irreversible Binomials in Neglect Dyslexia.Giorgio Arcara, Graziano Lacaita, Elisa Mattaloni, Laura Passarini, Sara Mondini, Paola Benincà & Carlo Semenza - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.details
|
|