Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Connectionist value units: Some concerns.John A. Barnden - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):92-93.
    This paper is a commentary on the target article by Dana H. Ballard, “Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function”, in the same issue of the journal, pp. 67–120. -/- I raise some issues about the connectionist or neural-network implementation of information and information processing. Issues include the sharing of information by different parts of a connectionist/neural network, the copying of complex information from one place to another in a network, the possibility of connection weights not being synaptic weights, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Replies to comments.Patricia Smith Churchland - 1986 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 29 (1-4):241 – 272.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Control, connectionism and cognition: Towards a new regulatory paradigm.C. A. Hooker, H. B. Penfold & R. J. Evans - 1992 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 43 (4):517-536.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Mind architecture and brain architecture.Camilo J. Cela-Conde & Gisèle Marty - 1997 - Biology and Philosophy 12 (3):327-340.
    The use of the computer metaphor has led to the proposal of mind architecture (Pylyshyn 1984; Newell 1990) as a model of the organization of the mind. The dualist computational model, however, has, since the earliest days of psychological functionalism, required that the concepts mind architecture and brain architecture be remote from each other. The development of both connectionism and neurocomputational science, has sought to dispense with this dualism and provide general models of consciousness – a uniform cognitive architecture –, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Value, variable, and coarse coding by posterior parietal neurons.Richard A. Andersen - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):90-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Brain metaphors, theories, and facts.Stephen Grossberg - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):97-98.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognition and conceptual change: A reply to double.Paul M. Churchland - 1986 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 16 (2):217–221.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • (2 other versions)Problems in the Development of Cognitive Neuroscience Effective Communication between Scientific Domains.Edward Manier - 1986 - PSA Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986 (1):183-197.
    Could anything provide a philosophically convincing mark of the mental in simple organisms (Lloyd 1984)? Individual organisms’ capacities to modify behavior adaptively as a result of past encounters with the environment might mark the first step in the phylogeny of minds. The simplest examples of mental representation are likely to be found in the simplest forms of animal learning.The most scientifically rigorous test case of “bottom- up” strategies in cognitive neuroscience is provided by current studies of the cellular and molecular (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spaces in the Brain: From Neurons to Meanings.Christian Balkenius & Peter Gärdenfors - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Spaces in the brain can refer either to psychological spaces, which are derived from similarity judgments, or to neurocognitive spaces, which are based on the activities of neural structures. We want to show how psychological spaces naturally emerge from the underlying neural spaces by dimension reductions that preserve similarity structures and the relevant categorizations. Some neuronal representational formats that may generate the psychological spaces are presented, compared and discussed in relation to the mathematical principles of monotonicity, continuity and convexity. In (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Towards a theory of cognition under a new control paradigm.C. A. Hooker, H. B. Penfold & R. J. Evans - 1992 - Topoi 11 (1):71-88.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Folk psychology as theory or practice? The case for eliminative materialism.John M. Preston - 1989 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 32 (September):277-303.
    One foundation of Eliminative Materialism is the claim that the totality of our ordinary resources for explaining and predicting behaviour, ?Folk Psychology?, constitutes a theoretical scheme, potentially in conflict with other theories of behaviour. Recent attacks upon this claim, as well as the defence by Paul Churchland, are examined and found to be lacking in a suitably realistic conception of theory. By finding such a conception, and by correctly identifying the level of conceptual structures within which Folk Psychology is located, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • What's in the term connectionist?.Christof Koch - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-101.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Multiple realization and methodological pluralism.Robert C. Richardson - 2009 - Synthese 167 (3):473-492.
    Multiple realization was once taken to be a challenge to reductionist visions, especially within cognitive science, and a foundation of the “antireductionist consensus.” More recently, multiple realization has come to be challenged on naturalistic grounds, as well as on more “metaphysical” grounds. Within cognitive science, one focal issue concerns the role of neural plasticity for addressing these issues. If reorganization maintains the same cognitive functions, that supports claims for multiple realization. I take up the reorganization involved in language dysfunctions to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Value encoding of patterns and variable encoding of transformations?John C. Baird - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):91-92.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Continuity of Philosophy and the Sciences.Paul M. Churchland - 1986 - Mind and Language 1 (1):5-14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The gap from sensation to cognition.Michael S. Landy - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):101-102.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • What does the cortex do?Mriganka Sur - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):105-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):67-90.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • Mental representation from the bottom up.Dan Lloyd - 1987 - Synthese 70 (January):23-78.
    Commonsense psychology and cognitive science both regularly assume the existence of representational states. I propose a naturalistic theory of representation sufficient to meet the pretheoretical constraints of a "folk theory of representation", constraints including the capacities for accuracy and inaccuracy, selectivity of proper objects of representation, perspective, articulation, and "efficacy" or content-determined functionality. The proposed model states that a representing device is a device which changes state when information is received over multiple information channels originating at a single source. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Mental Representation, Conceptual Spaces and Metaphors.Peter Gärdenfors - 1996 - Synthese 106 (1):21 - 47.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • “Grandmother networks” and computational economy.J. J. Hopfield - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):100-100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Old dogmas and new axioms in brain theory.Andràs J. Pellionisz - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):103-104.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Connectionist computing and neural machinery: Examining the test of “timing”.John K. Tsotsos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):106-107.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does the brain compute?Erich Harth - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):98-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Computational neuroscience.Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):104-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Two tests for the value unit model: Multicell recordings and pointers.David Mumford - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):102-103.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Invariant and programmable neuropsychological systems are fibrations.William C. Hoffman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):99-100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cortical architectures and value unit encoding.Charles D. Gilbert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):96-97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Abstract solutions versus neurobiologically plausible problems.Jeffrey Foss - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):95-96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What's the connection?Leif H. Finkel & George N. Reeke - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):94-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Phase-space representation and coordinate transformation: A general paradigm for neural computation.Paul M. Churchland - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):93-94.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Value units make the right connections.Dana H. Ballard - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):107-120.
    The cerebral cortex is a rich and diverse structure that is the basis of intelligent behavior. One of the deepest mysteries of the function of cortex is that neural processing times are only about one hundred times as fast as the fastest response times for complex behavior. At the very least, this would seem to indicate that the cortex does massive amounts of parallel computation.This paper explores the hypothesis that an important part of the cortex can be modeled as a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation