Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Re-establishing the distinction between numerosity, numerousness, and number in numerical cognition.César Frederico Dos Santos - 2022 - Philosophical Psychology 35 (8):1152-1180.
    In 1939, the influential psychophysicist S. S. Stevens proposed definitional distinctions between the terms ‘number,’ ‘numerosity,’ and ‘numerousness.’ Although the definitions he proposed were adopted by syeveral psychophysicists and experimental psychologists in the 1940s and 1950s, they were almost forgotten in the subsequent decades, making room for what has been described as a “terminological chaos” in the field of numerical cognition. In this paper, I review Stevens’s distinctions to help bring order to this alleged chaos and to shed light on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Varieties of numerical abilities.Stanislas Dehaene - 1992 - Cognition 44 (1-2):1-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   113 citations  
  • Précis of the number sense.Stanislas Dehaene - 2001 - Mind and Language 16 (1):16–36.
    ‘Number sense’ is a short‐hand for our ability to quickly understand, approximate, and manipulate numerical quantities. My hypothesis is that number sense rests on cerebral circuits that have evolved specifically for the purpose of representing basic arithmetic knowledge. Four lines of evidence suggesting that number sense constitutes a domain‐specific, biologically‐determined ability are reviewed: the presence of evolutionary precursors of arithmetic in animals; the early emergence of arithmetic competence in infants independently of other abilities, including language; the existence of a homology (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • The risks of rationalising cognitive development.Beatrice de Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):713-714.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Towards a Darwinian approach to mathematics.Helen De Cruz - 2006 - Foundations of Science 11 (1-2):157-196.
    In the past decades, recent paradigm shifts in ethology, psychology, and the social sciences have given rise to various new disciplines like cognitive ethology and evolutionary psychology. These disciplines use concepts and theories of evolutionary biology to understand and explain the design, function and origin of the brain. I shall argue that there are several good reasons why this approach could also apply to human mathematical abilities. I will review evidence from various disciplines (cognitive ethology, cognitive psychology, cognitive archaeology and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • What defines a legitimate issue for Skinnerian psychology: Philosophy or technology?Hank Davis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):137-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rats counting rats: The use of conspecifics as discriminative stimuli.Hank Davis & Laurie Hiestand - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (5):356-358.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • No report; no feeling.Lawrence H. Davis - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):647-648.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is there a comparative psychology of implicit mathematical knowledge?Hank Davis - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):250-250.
    Geary suggests that implicit mathematical principles exist across human cultures and transcend sex differences. Is such knowledge present in animals as well, and is it sufficient to account for performance in all species, including our own? I attempt to trace the implications of Gearys target article for comparative psychology, questioning the exclusion of “subitizing” in describing human mathematical performance, and asking whether human researchers function as cultural agents with animals, elevating their implicit knowledge to secondary domains of numerical performance.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Redescribing redescription.Terry Dartnall - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):712-713.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Towards a Darwinian Approach to Mathematics.Helen Cruz - 2006 - Foundations of Science 11 (1):157-196.
    In the past decades, recent paradigm shifts in ethology, psychology, and the social sciences have given rise to various new disciplines like cognitive ethology and evolutionary psychology. These disciplines use concepts and theories of evolutionary biology to understand and explain the design, function and origin of the brain. I shall argue that there are several good reasons why this approach could also apply to human mathematical abilities. I will review evidence from various disciplines (cognitive ethology, cognitive psychology, cognitive archaeology and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • All sex differences in cognitive ability may be explained by an X-Y homologous gene determining degrees of cerebral asymmetry.T. J. Crow - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):249-250.
    Male superiority in mathematical ability (along with female superiority in verbal fluency) may reflect the operation of an X-Y homologous gene (the right-shift-factor) influencing the relative rates of development of the cerebral hemispheres. Alleles at the locus on the Y chromosome will be selected at a later mean age than alleles on the X, and only by females.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Arthropod Intelligence? The Case for Portia.Fiona R. Cross, Georgina E. Carvell, Robert R. Jackson & Randolph C. Grace - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Macphail’s ‘null hypothesis’, that there are no differences in intelligence, qualitative or quantitative, between non-human vertebrates has been controversial. This controversy can be useful if it encourages interest in acquiring a detailed understanding of how non-human animals express flexible problem-solving capacity (‘intelligence’), but limiting the discussion to vertebrates is too arbitrary. As an example, we focus here on Portia, a spider with an especially intricate predatory strategy and a preference for other spiders as prey. We review research on pre-planned detours, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Still far too sexy a topic.Susan F. Chipman - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):248-249.
    Geary is highly selective in his use of the literature on gender differences. His assumption of consistent female inferiority in mathematics is not necessarily supported by the facts.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal concepts: Content and discontent.Nick Chater & Cecilia Heyes - 1994 - Mind and Language 9 (3):209-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Do gender differences in spatial skills mediate gender differences in mathematics among high-ability students?M. Beth Casey - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):247-248.
    Based on Geary's theory, intelligence may determine which males utilize innate spatial knowledge to inform their mathematical solutions. This may explain why math gender differences occur mainly with higher abilities. In support, we found that mental rotation ability served as a mediator of gender differences on the math Scholastic Assessment Test for two high-ability samples. Our research suggests, however, that environment and biology interact to influence mental rotation abilities.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representational redescription and cognitive architectures.Antonella Carassa & Maurizio Tirassa - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):711-712.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Representational redescription and cognitive architectures.Antonella Carassa & Maurizio Tirassa - 1994 - Carassa, Antonella and Tirassa, Maurizio (1994) Representational Redescription and Cognitive Architectures. [Journal (Paginated)] 17 (4):711-712.
    We focus on Karmiloff-Smith's Representational redescription model, arguing that it poses some problems concerning the architecture of a redescribing system. To discuss the topic, we consider the implicit/explicit dichotomy and the relations between natur al language and the language of thought. We argue that the model regards how knowledge is employed rather than how it is represented in the system.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Plausible reconstruction? No!E. J. Capaldi & Robert W. Proctor - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):646-647.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What's getting redescribed?Robert L. Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):710-711.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A test for S-S associations in a conditional counting task.Richard A. Burns & Frances A. Nesbitt - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (5):441-444.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal communication of private states does not illuminate the human case.Selmer Bringsjord & Elizabeth Bringsjord - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):645-646.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Validation of behavioural equations: Can neurobiology help?C. M. Bradshaw - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):136-137.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The outside route to the inside story.Marc N. Branch - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):644-645.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The response problem.Robert C. Bolles - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):135-136.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Representational redescription: A question of sequence.Margaret A. Boden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):708-708.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A Fodorian guide to Switzerland: Jung and Piaget combined?Péter Bodor & Csaba Pléh - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):709-710.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The real problem with constructivism.Paul Bloom & Karen Wynn - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):707-708.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The status of private events in behavior analysis.William M. Baum - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):644-644.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Communication and internal states: What is their relationship?Michael Bamberg - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):643-644.
    Common folks “have” emotions and talk to others; and sometimes they make “their” emotions the topic of such talk. The emotions seem to be “theirs,” since they can be conceived of as private states ; and they can be topicalized, because we seem to be able to attribute or lend a conventionalized public form to some inner state or event. This is the way much of our folk-talk and folk-thinking about emotions, the expression thereof, the role of language in these (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perhaps Sisyphus is the relevant model for animal-language researchers.Donald M. Baer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):642-643.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate.Christian Agrillo & Angelo Bisazza - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Number versus continuous quantity in numerosity judgments by fish.Christian Agrillo, Laura Piffer & Angelo Bisazza - 2011 - Cognition 119 (2):281-287.
    In quantity discrimination tasks, adults, infants and animals have been sometimes observed to process number only after all continuous variables, such as area or density, have been controlled for. This has been taken as evidence that processing number may be more cognitively demanding than processing continuous variables. We tested this hypothesis by training mosquitofish to discriminate two items from three in three different conditions. In one condition, continuous variables were controlled while numerical information was available; in another, the number was (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Numbers and Arithmetic: Neither Hardwired Nor Out There.Rafael Núñez - 2009 - Biological Theory 4 (1):68-83.
    What is the nature of number systems and arithmetic that we use in science for quantification, analysis, and modeling? I argue that number concepts and arithmetic are neither hardwired in the brain, nor do they exist out there in the universe. Innate subitizing and early cognitive preconditions for number— which we share with many other species—cannot provide the foundations for the precision, richness, and range of number concepts and simple arithmetic, let alone that of more complex mathematical concepts. Numbers and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • What's the stimulus?G. E. Zuriff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):664-664.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genetic influences on sex differences in outstanding mathematical reasoning ability.Ada H. Zohar - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):266-267.
    Sexual selection provides an adequate partial explanation for the difference in means between the distributions, but fails to explain the difference in variance, that is, the overrepresentation of both boys with outstanding mathematical reasoning ability and boys with mental retardation. Other genetic factors are probably at work. While spatial ability is correlated with OMRA, so are other cognitive abilities. OMRA is not reducible to spatial ability; hence selection for navigational skill is unlikely to be the only mechanism by which males (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The assessment of intentionality in animals.Thomas R. Zentall - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):663-663.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From the decline of development to the ascent of consciousness.Philip David Zelazo - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):731-732.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The return of the reinforcement theorists.C. D. L. Wynne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):156-156.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sex differences and evolutionary by-products.Thomas Wynn, Forrest Tierson & Craig Palmer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):265-266.
    From the perspective of evolutionary theory, we believe it makes more sense to view the sex differences in spatial cognition as being an evolutionary by-product of selection for optimal rates of fetal development. Geary does not convince us that his proposed selective factors operated with “sufficient precision, economy, and efficiency.” Moreover, the archaeological evidence does not support his proposed evolutionary scenario.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants.Karen Wynn - 1998 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (8):296-303.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • A mathematical theory of reinforcement: An unexpected place to find support for analogical memory coding.Donald M. Wilkie & Lisa M. Saksida - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-156.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fifty years on: The new “principles of behavior”?J. H. Wearden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):155-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How general is a general theory of reinforcement?Stephen F. Walker - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):154-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is there an implicit level of representation?Annie Vinter & Pierre Perruchet - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):730-731.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Animal-centered models of reinforcement.William Timberlake - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):153-154.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Between-sex differences are often averaging artifacts.Hoben Thomas - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):265-265.
    The central problem in Geary's theory is how differences are conceptualized. Recent research has shown that between-sex differences on certain tasks are a consequence of averaging within sex differences. A mixture distribution models between-sex differences on several tasks well and does not appear congenial to a sexual-selection perspective.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are some mental states public events?Nicholas S. Thompson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):662-663.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • We are far from understanding sex-related differences in spatial-mathematical abilities despite the theory of sexual selection.Üner Tan - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):264-264.
    I have provided evidence that Geary's model does not explain male dominance in spatial abilities by sexual selection. The current literature concerning the relations of nonverbal IQ to testosterone, hand preference, and right- and left-hand skill, as well as the organizing effects of testosterone on cerebral lateralization during the perinatal period, does not support Geary's arguments.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Short-term memory in human operant conditioning.Frode Svartdal - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):152-153.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark