Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Majority group children expect that ethnic out-group peers feel fewer positive but more negative emotions than in-group peers.Jellie Sierksma & Gijsbert Bijlstra - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (6):1210-1223.
    ABSTRACTAcross two studies majority group children’s perception of positive and negative emotions in ethnic in-group and disadvantaged ethnic out-group peers was examined. Study 1 (N =...
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What we would (but shouldn't) do for those we love: Universalism versus partiality in responding to others' moral transgressions.Laura K. Soter, Martha K. Berg, Susan A. Gelman & Ethan Kross - 2021 - Cognition 217 (C):104886.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Development of Intergroup Cooperation: Children Show Impartial Fairness and Biased Care.John Corbit, Hayley MacDougall, Stef Hartlin & Chris Moore - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    One of the most remarkable features of human societies is our ability to cooperate with each other. However, the benefits of cooperation are not extended to everyone. Indeed, another hallmark of human societies is a division between us and them. Favoritism toward members of our group can result in a loss of empathy and greater tolerance of harm toward those outside our group. The current study sought to investigate how in-group bias impacts the developmental emergence of concerns for fairness and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preschoolers Favor Their Ingroup When Resources Are Limited.Kristy Jia Jin Lee, Gianluca Esposito & Peipei Setoh - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:398351.
    The present study examined how two- to four-year-old preschoolers in Singapore (N = 202) balance fairness and ingroup loyalty in resource distribution. Specifically, we investigated whether children would enact fair distributions as defined by an equality rule, or show partiality toward their ingroup when distributing resources, and the conditions under which one distributive strategy may take precedence over the other. In Experiment 1, children distributed four different pairs of toys between two puppets. In the Group condition, one puppet was assigned (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Origins of Intergroup Resource Inequality Influence Children’s Decision to Perpetuate or Rectify Inequality.Jing An, Jing Yu & Liqi Zhu - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Previous studies have explored children’s intergroup resource allocation in the context of preexisting intergroup resource inequality. However, resource inequality between social groups often originates from different factors. This study explored the role of the origins of resource inequality on children’s intergroup resource allocations. In experiment 1, when there was no explicit origin of the intergroup inequality, children of different ages mainly allocated resources in an equal way and 5- to 6-year-olds showed ingroup bias. In experiment 2, we examined the influence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The origins of morality: Social equality, fairness, and justice.Melanie Killen - 2018 - Philosophical Psychology 31 (5):767-803.
    Tomasello’s A Natural History of Morality is novel, compelling, and comprehensive. Drawing on past and current research in developmental psychology, as well as moral philosophy, I make the following points: (1) cooperation is a significant major hallmark of early human sociality but is also the foundation for antagonistic goals designed to enhance one’s own group’s benefit at the cost of due justice to others; (2) interdependence coexists with independent autonomous thinking, which is necessary for challenging group norms, authority, and institutional (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation