Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. (1 other version)Do deaf individuals see better?Daphne Bavelier, Matthew W. G. Dye & Peter C. Hauser - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (11):512-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • The cognitive functions of language.Peter Carruthers - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (6):657-674.
    This paper explores a variety of different versions of the thesis that natural language is involved in human thinking. It distinguishes amongst strong and weak forms of this thesis, dismissing some as implausibly strong and others as uninterestingly weak. Strong forms dismissed include the view that language is conceptually necessary for thought (endorsed by many philosophers) and the view that language is _de facto_ the medium of all human conceptual thinking (endorsed by many philosophers and social scientists). Weak forms include (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   151 citations  
  • Space rotation, perspective shift, and verb morphology in ASL.Terry Janzen - 2004 - Cognitive Linguistics 15 (2).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Visible Social Interactions Do Not Support the Development of False Belief Understanding in the Absence of Linguistic Input: Evidence from Deaf Adult Homesigners.Deanna L. Gagne & Marie Coppola - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The moral case for sign language education.Julian Savulescu, Angela Morgan, Christopher Gyngell & Hilary Bowman-Smart - 2019 - Monash Bioethics Review 37 (3-4):94-110.
    Here, a moral case is presented as to why sign languages such as Auslan should be made compulsory in general school curricula. Firstly, there are significant benefits that accrue to individuals from learning sign language. Secondly, sign language education is a matter of justice; the normalisation of sign language education and use would particularly benefit marginalised groups, such as those living with a communication disability. Finally, the integration of sign languages into the curricula would enable the flourishing of Deaf culture (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • (1 other version)Do deaf individuals see better?Peter C. Hauser Daphne Bavelier, Matthew W. G. Dye - 2006 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (11):512.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Modifications of Visual Field Asymmetries for Face Categorization in Early Deaf Adults: A Study With Chimeric Faces.Marjorie Dole, David Méary & Olivier Pascalis - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Language aptitude in the visuospatial modality: L2 British Sign Language acquisition and cognitive skills in British Sign Language-English interpreting students.Freya Watkins, Stacey Webb, Christopher Stone & Robin L. Thompson - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Sign language interpreting is a cognitively challenging task performed mostly by second language learners. SLI students must first gain language fluency in a new visuospatial modality and then move between spoken and signed modalities as they interpret. As a result, many students plateau before reaching working fluency, and SLI training program drop-out rates are high. However, we know little about the requisite skills to become a successful interpreter: the few existing studies investigating SLI aptitude in terms of linguistic and cognitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning an Embodied Visual Language: Four Imitation Strategies Available to Sign Learners.Aaron Shield & Richard P. Meier - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark