Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A theory of eye movements during target acquisition.Gregory J. Zelinsky - 2008 - Psychological Review 115 (4):787-835.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Top-down effect on pupillary response: Evidence from shape from shading.Ayelet Sapir, Ronen Hershman & Avishai Henik - 2021 - Cognition 212 (C):104664.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preemption effects in visual search: Evidence for low-level grouping.Ronald A. Rensink & James T. Enns - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (1):101-130.
    Experiments are presented showing that visual search for Mueller-Lyer (ML) stimuli is based on complete configurations, rather than component segments. Segments easily detected in isolation were difficult to detect when embedded in a configuration, indicating preemption by low-level groups. This preemption—which caused stimulus components to become inaccessible to rapid search—was an all-or-nothing effect, and so could serve as a powerful test of grouping. It is shown that these effects are unlikely to be due to blurring by simple spatial filters at (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Superior modulation of activation levels of stimulus representations does not underlie superior discrimination in autism.Michelle O'Riordan - 2000 - Cognition 77 (2):81-96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Superior modulation of activation levels of stimulus representations does not underlie superior discrimination in autism.Michelle O'Riordan - 2000 - Cognition 77 (2):81-96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attention capture by faces.Stephen R. H. Langton, Anna S. Law, A. Mike Burton & Stefan R. Schweinberger - 2008 - Cognition 107 (1):330-342.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Can visual cognitive neuroscience learn anything from the philosophy of language? Ambiguity and the topology of neural network models of multistable perception.Philipp Koralus - 2016 - Synthese 193 (5):1409-1432.
    The Necker cube and the productive class of related stimuli involving multiple depth interpretations driven by corner-like line junctions are often taken to be ambiguous. This idea is normally taken to be as little in need of defense as the claim that the Necker cube gives rise to multiple distinct percepts. In the philosophy of language, it is taken to be a substantive question whether a stimulus that affords multiple interpretations is a case of ambiguity. If we take into account (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Salience of visual parts.Donald D. Hoffman & Manish Singh - 1997 - Cognition 63 (1):29-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Attention, spatial representation, and visual neglect: Simulating emergent attention and spatial memory in the selective attention for identification model (SAIM).Dietmar Heinke & Glyn W. Humphreys - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (1):29-87.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Asymmetric visual representation of sex from human body shape.Marco Gandolfo & Paul E. Downing - 2020 - Cognition 205 (C):104436.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Competition for consciousness among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual processes.Vincent Di Lollo, James T. Enns & Ronald A. Rensink - 2000 - Journal Of Experimental Psychology-General 129 (4):481-507.
    Advances in neuroscience implicate reentrant signaling as the predominant form of communication between brain areas. This principle was used in a series of masking experiments that defy explanation by feed-forward theories. The masking occurs when a brief display of target plus mask is continued with the mask alone. Two masking processes were found: an early process affected by physical factors such as adapting luminance and a later process affected by attentional factors such as set size. This later process is called (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   73 citations  
  • The world, the brain, and the speed of sight.Ronald A. Rensink - 1996 - In David Knill & Whitman Richards (eds.), Perception as Bayesian Inference. Cambridge University Press. pp. 495-498.
    Adelson & Pentland (Chapter 11) use an engaging metaphor to illustrate their position on scene analysis: interpretations are produced by a workshop that employs a set of specialists, each concerned with a single aspect of the scene. The authors argue that it is too expensive to have a supervisor co-ordinate the specialists and that it is too expensive to let them operate independently. They then show that a careful sequencing of the specialists leads to solutions of minimum cost, at least (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Early completion of occluded objects.Ronald A. Rensink & James T. Enns - 1998 - Vision Research 38:2489-2505.
    We show that early vision can use monocular cues to rapidly complete partially-occluded objects. Visual search for easily detected fragments becomes difficult when the completed shape is similar to others in the display; conversely, search for fragments that are difficult to detect becomes easy when the completed shape is distinctive. Results indicate that completion occurs via the occlusion-triggered removal of occlusion edges and linking of associated regions. We fail to find evidence for a visible filling-in of contours or surfaces, but (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Changes.Ronald A. Rensink - 2002 - Progress in Brain Research 140:199-209.
    This past decade has seen a great resurgence of interest in the perception of change. Change has, of course, long been recognized as a phenomenon worthy of study, and vision scientists have given their attention to it at various times in the past (for a review, see Rensink, 2002a). But things seem different this time around. This time, there is an emerging belief that instead of being just another visual ability, the perception of change may be something central to our (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The dynamic representation of scenes.Ronald A. Rensink - 2000 - Visual Cognition 7 (1/2/3):17-42.
    One of the more powerful impressions created by vision is that of a coherent, richly-detailed world where everything is present simultaneously. Indeed, this impression is so compelling that we tend to ascribe these properties not only to the external world, but to our internal representations as well. But results from several recent experiments argue against this latter ascription. For example, changes in images of real-world scenes often go unnoticed when made during a saccade, flicker, blink, or movie cut. This "change (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Ideal observers, real observers, and the return of Elvis.Ronald A. Rensink - 1996 - In David Knill & Whitman Richards (eds.), Perception as Bayesian Inference. Cambridge University Press. pp. 451-455.
    Knill, Kersten, & Mamassian (Chapter 6) provide an interesting discussion of how the Bayesian formulation can be used to help investigate human vision. In their view, computational theories can be based on an ideal observer that uses Bayesian inference to make optimal use of available information. Four factors are important here: the image information used, the output structures estimated, the priors assumed (i.e., knowledge about the structure of the world), and the likelihood function used (i.e., knowledge about the projection of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visual Attention.Jeremy Wolfe - 2000 - In K.K. De Valois (ed.), Seeing. Academic Press. pp. 335-386.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Perception preattentive and phenomenal.Austen Clark - manuscript
    Recent work in experimental psychology and neuroscience has revealed a rather surprising architecture for early (or preattentive) perceptual processes. This paper will describe some of the surprising features of that architecture, and how they bear on recent philosophical debates about the notion of phenomenal consciousness. I will argue that the common sense idea that states of phenomenal consciousness are states of a unitary kind cannot survive confrontation with the details of how our early perceptual processing works. In particular, that architecture (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Visual Attention and Temporal Binding.Frank Bauer - unknown
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Weighting Mechanisms Within and Across Modalities.Thomas Töllner - unknown
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Role of facilitatory and inhibitory short-term memory mechanisms for the guidance of visual search.Thomas Geyer - unknown
    In the visual search paradigm, participants’ task is to detect the presence or absence of a target item, which is presented in an array of distractor items. Usually it is found that performance is dependent on specific properties of the visual display, for example, the number of items to be searched or the similarity between display items. However, recent research has demonstrated that memory mechanisms can also affect search behaviour. Further, it was found that memory mechanisms can, in principle, be (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preattentive precursors to phenomenal properties.Austen Clark - manuscript
    What are the relations between preattentive feature-placing and states of perceptual awareness? For the purposes of this paper, states of "perceptual awareness" are confined to the simplest possible exemplars: states in which one is aware of some aspect of the appearance of something one perceives. Subjective contours are used as an example. Early visual processing seems to employ independent, high-bandwidth, preattentive feature "channels", followed by a selective process that directs selective attention. The mechanisms that yield subjective contours are found very (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark