Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. A gesture in the right direction?Michael C. Corballis - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):697-697.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Conceptual structure and syntax.Frederick J. Newmeyer - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):202-202.
    The syntactic structures of natural languages reflect conceptual categories more directly than they reflect communicative categories. This fact supports the main premise of the target article, namely, that the most important event in language evolution was the development of a hierarchical conceptual structure.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A case for auditory temporal processing as an evolutionary precursor to speech processing and language function.Roslyn Holly Fitch & Paula Tallal - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):189-189.
    Wilkins & Wakefield suggest that changes in the hominid brain made it uniquely “preadaptive” for language, yet no precursor functions served as adaptive substrates to the emergence of language. We present contrary evidence that the ability to discriminate and process rapid and complex auditory information is a cross-species function subserving communication processes including, but not limited to, human speech perception. We suggest that auditory temporal processing served as an evolutionary precursor to speech processing and consequent language development in humans.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brains evolution and neurolinguistic preconditions.Wendy K. Wilkins & Jennie Wakefield - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):161-182.
    This target article presents a plausible evolutionary scenario for the emergence of the neural preconditions for language in the hominid lineage. In pleistocene primate lineages there was a paired evolutionary expansion of frontal and parietal neocortex (through certain well-documented adaptive changes associated with manipulative behaviors) resulting, in ancestral hominids, in an incipient Broca's region and in a configurationally unique junction of the parietal, occipital, and temporal lobes of the brain (the POT). On our view, the development of the POT in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Issues in neo- and paleoneurology of language.Harry J. Jerison - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):195-196.
    Wilkins and Wakefield's hypothesis that language is fundamentally a cognitive rather than cominunicational adaptation is reasonable, but there are flaws in their anatomical and fossil evidence. Their analysis of reorganization also needs clarification. Finally, the origin of language ability must have occurred with australopithecine rather than habiline adaptations on entry into the novel hominid adaptive zone.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Did primates need more than social grooming and increased group size for acquiring language?Jan Wind - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):720-720.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do gossip and lack of grooming make us human?Ilya I. Glezer & Warren G. Kinzey - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):704-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anthropological criticisms of Dunbar's theory of the origin of language.Robert Bates Graber - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):705-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neurolinguistic models and fossil reconstructions.Merlin Donald - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):188-189.
    Hominid-like morphology in habiline cranial endocasts does not necessarily imply the presence of language capacity. The cortical zone in question is not associated exclusively with language in humans, and its emergence in habilines might indicate the evolution of other cognitive functions special to humans that were preconditions for the later evolution of language.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Finding the true place of Homo habilis in language evolution.Derek Bickerton - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):182-183.
    Despite some sound basic assumptions, Wilkins & Wakefield portray a Homo habilis too linguistically sophisticated to fit in with the subsequent fossil record and thereby lose a reasoned explanation for human innovativeness. They err, too, in accepting a single-level model of conceptual structure and in deriving initial linguistic units from calls, a process far more dubious than the derivation of home-sign from naive gesture.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bartering old stone tools: When did communicative ability and conceptual structure begin to interact?Stephen F. Walker - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):203-204.
    Wilkins & Wakefield are clearly right to separate linguistic capacity from communicative ability, if only because other animal species have one without the other. But I question the abruptness of the demarcation they make between a period when hominids evolved enriched conceptual representation for other reasons entirely, and a subsequent later stage when language use became an adaptation.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do grooming and speech really serve homologous functions?Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):700-701.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Human language: Are nonhuman precursors lacking?Marc D. Hauser & Nathan D. Wolfea - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):190-191.
    Contra Wilkins & Wakefield, we argue that an evolutionarily inspired approach to language must consider different facets of language (i.e., more than syntax and semantics), and must explore the possibility of nonhuman precursors. Several examples are discussed, illustrating the power of the comparative approach in illuminating our understanding of language evolution.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Semiogenesis as a continuous, not a discrete, phenomenon.Jo Liska - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):198-199.
    This commentary confronts one of the central tenets advanced in Wilkins & Wakefield's target article: By adopting a very narrow perspective on language, the authors have effectively limited discussion of earlier linguistic capabilities thought to be at least facilitative of, if not prerequisite to language defined as a An alternative conceptualization for describing semiogenesis is offered.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The functions of grooming and language: The present need not reflect the past.Marc Hauser, Leah Gardner, Tony Goldberg & Adrian Treves - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):706-707.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Confounded correlations, again.Terrence W. Deacon - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):698-699.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Comparative studies, phylogenies and predictions of coevolutionary relationships.Emília P. Martins - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):714-716.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Single words, multiple words, and the functions of language.A. Charles Catania - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):184-185.
    Wilkins & Wakefield assign importance to motor systems but skip from anatomy to cognitive structure with little attention to behavior. Organisms, no matter how sophisticated, that do not behave in accord with what they know will fall by the evolutionary wayside. Facts about behavior can supplement the authors' theory, whose hierarchical structures can accommodate an evolutionary scenario in which a million years or more of functionally varied utterances mainly limited to single words is followed by an explosion of linguistic diversity (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans.R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):681-694.
    Group size is a function of relative neocortical volume in nonhuman primates. Extrapolation from this regression equation yields a predicted group size for modern humans very similar to that of certain hunter-gatherer and traditional horticulturalist societies. Groups of similar size are also found in other large-scale forms of contemporary and historical society. Among primates, the cohesion of groups is maintained by social grooming; the time devoted to social grooming is linearly related to group size among the Old World monkeys and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   215 citations  
  • Number our days: Quantifying social evolution.Harry J. Jerison - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):712-713.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavioural constraints on social communication are not likely to prevent the evolution of large social groups in nonhuman primates.R. J. Andrew - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):694-694.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Issues and nonissues in the origins of language.Wendy K. Wilkins & Jennie Wakefield - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):205-226.
    This response clarifies the nature of reappropriation and the definition of language. It explicates the relationship between neural systems and language and between homology and evolutionary gradualism. Through a review of ape capacities in the realms of language and tool use, it distinguishes human language acquisition from nonhuman learning. Finally, it suggests the appropriate sorts of evidence on which to base further evolutionary arguments relevant to the origins of language.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the origins of language: A history of constraints and windows of opportunity.R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):721-735.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Size of human groups during the Paleolithic and the evolutionary significance of increased group size.Michael E. Hyland - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):709-710.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Complex behaviors: Evolution and the brain.William O. Dingwall - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):186-188.
    Three issues are addressed in this commentary. (1) Wilkins & Wakefield are commended for placing the complex behavior they discuss within an evolutionary matrix. (2) They err on a number of points in regard to their treatment of this complex behavior. These involve (a) their emphasis on the evolution of conceptual structure rather than language, (b) their equation of meaning with reference, (c) their minimalist view of learning theory, and (d) their separation of the evolution of speech from that of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evidence for POT expansion in early Homo: A pretty theory with ugly (or no) paleoneurological facts.Ralph L. Holloway - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):191-193.
    If POT (parieto-occipital-temporal junction) reorganization came earlier in australopithecines than in Homo, it is likely that the selective pressures were different, and not necessarily directed toward language. The brain endocast evidence for the POT in A. afarensis is actually better than it is for early Homo.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Group size, language and evolutionary mechanisms.Harold Kincaid - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):713-714.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Vocal grooming: Man the schmoozer.David Dean - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):699-700.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Mosaic evolution of the neocortex.Dean Falk & Bruce Dudek - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):701-702.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Do larger brains mean greater intelligence?R. W. Byrne - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):696-697.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The hominid tool-language connection: Some missing evolutionary links?A. Maryanski - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):199-200.
    This commentary criticizes Wilkins & Wakefield's thesis that the neurological precursors of language provide a cognitive Rubicon to linguistically divide human from nonhuman primates. A causal model of their theory is presented, followed by a discussion of the relationship between brain expansion and tool use, Broca's area and the parietaloccipital-temporal junction (POT).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lending a hand.Michael C. Corballis - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):185-186.
    The precise manner in which language serves its communicative function suggests that natural selection, rather than exaptation or reappropriation, played the major role in its evolution. Natural selection is more readily invoked, I suggest, if it is assumed that language originated as a system of manual gestures, and later switched to an oral mode.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A developmental look at grooming, grunting and group cohesion.Lorraine McCune - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):716-717.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brains, grouping and language.A. H. Harcourt - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):706-706.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Coming of age in Olduvai and the Zaire rain forest.Justin Leiber - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):196-197.
    ProbablyHomo habilisis two species not one; similarly for Pan troglodytes. Although amenable to training, in naturePan paniscusmay be a “specialized insular dwarf.” Language is uniquely human, but symbolic behavior and intelligence are widespread among animals with little respect for phylogenetic closeness toHomo sapiens.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language and levels of selection.Lee Alan Dugatkin & David Sloan Wilson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):701-701.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grooming and language as cohesion mechanisms: Choosing the right data.Marina Cords - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):697-698.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brain expansion: Thoughts on hunting or reckoning kinship – or both?C. Loring Brace - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):695-696.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Manual versus speech motor control and the evolution of language.Philip Lieberman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):197-198.
    Inferences made from endocasts of fossil skulls cannot provide information on the function of particular neocortical areas or the subcortical pathways to prefrontal cortex that form part of the neural substrate for speech, syntax, and certain aspects of cognition. The neural bases of syntax cannot be disassociated from “communication.” Manual motor control was probably a preadaptive factor in the evolution of humansyntactic ability, but neurophysiological data on living humans show that speech motor control and syntax are more closely linked. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Apes and language: Human uniqueness again?Robert W. Mitchell & H. Lyn Miles - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):200-201.
    Wilkins & Wakefield's intriguing model of language evolution is deficient in evidence of human uniqueness in metaphorical matching, amodal representation, reference, conceptual structure, hierarchical organization, linguistic comprehension, sign use, laterality, and handedness. Primates show communicative reference, laterality, and handedness, and apes in particular show hierarchical organization, conceptual structure, cross-modal abilities, sign use, and displaced reference.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Social complexity: The roles of primates' grooming and people's talking.Andrew Whiten - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):719-719.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grooming is not the only regulator of primate social interactions.Robert M. Seyfarth & Dorothy L. Cheney - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):717-718.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The rest of the story: Grooming, group size and vocal exchanges in neotropical primates.Charles T. Snowdon - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):718-718.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Palaeoneurology of language: Grounds for scepticism.Elizabeth Whitcombe - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):204-205.
    Wilkins & Wakefield's identification of anatomical features in the Koobi Fora endocast, which may be thought to carry some functional significance in relation to organization for language, raises fundamental problems of method: attention is drawn to some limitations of the evidence, of endocasts and of the neuroanatomical map used to interpret them.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural preconditions for proto-language.James R. Hurford & Simon Kirby - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):193-194.
    Representation must be prior to communication in evolution. Wilkins & Wakefield's target article gives the impression that communicative pressures play a secondary role. We suggest that their evolutionary precursor is compatible with protolanguage rather than language itself. The difference between these two communicative systems should not be underestimated: only the former can be trivially reappropriated from a representational system.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language as a multimodal sensory enhancement system.Bob Jacobs & John M. Horner - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):194-195.
    Several claims made by Wilkins & Wakefield require qualification. First, the proposed delineation of the parietal-occipital-temporal junction (POT) is overly restrictive. Second, focusing exclusively on the evolutionary importance of manual manipulation oversimplifies interacting evolutionary preconditions for language. Finally, Wilkins and Wakefield's perspective adheres to a homocentric, formal, linguistic definition of language instead of viewing language as a multimodal sensory enhancement system unique to each species.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Primate group size, brains and communication: A New World perspective.Charles H. Janson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):711-712.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is preadaptation for language a necessary assumption?David J. Bryant - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):183-184.
    Preadaptation for language is an unnecessary assumption because intermediate stages of linguistic ability are possible and adaptive. Language could have evolved through gradual selection from structures exhibiting few features associated with modern structures. Without physical evidence pertaining to language ability in prehabilis hominids, it remains possible that selective pressures for language use preceded and necessitated modern neurolinguistic structures.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Stone tools and conceptual structure.James Steele - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):202-203.
    Understanding how conceptual structures inform stone tool production and use would help us resolve the issue of a pongid-hominid dichotomy in brain organisation and cognitive ability. Evidence from ideational apraxia suggests that the planning of linguistic and manipulative behaviours is not colocalized in homologous circuits. An alternative account in terms of the evolutionary expansion of the whole prefrontal-premotor area may be more plausible.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ecological and social variance and the evolution of increased neocortical size.R. A. Foley - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):702-703.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation