Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Precis of the modularity of mind.Jerry A. Fodor - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):1-42.
    The Modularity of Mind proposes an alternative to the or view of cognitive architecture that has dominated several decades of cognitive science. Whereas interactionism stresses the continuity of perceptual and cognitive processes, modularity theory argues for their distinctness. It is argued, in particular, that the apparent plausibility of New Look theorizing derives from the failure to distinguish between the (correct) claim that perceptual processes are inferential and the (dubious) claim that they are unencapsidated, that is, that they are arbitrarily sensitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   278 citations  
  • Keep the scope of neuroethology broad.James A. Simmons - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):400-401.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Disregarding vertebrates is neither useful nor necessary.Günter Ehret - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):385-386.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world.Christine A. Skarda & Walter J. Freeman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):161-173.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   447 citations  
  • Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?Emanuel Donchin & Michael G. H. Coles - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):357.
    To understand the endogenous components of the event-related brain potential (ERP), we must use data about the components' antecedent conditions to form hypotheses about the information-processing function of the underlying brain activity. These hypotheses, in turn, generate testable predictions about the consequences of the component. We review the application of this approach to the analysis of the P300 component. The amplitude of the P300 is controlled multiplicatively by the subjective probability and the task relevance of the eliciting events, whereas its (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   194 citations  
  • Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3.Rolf Verleger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):343.
    P3 is the most prominent of the electrical potentials of the human electroencephalogram that are sensitive to psychological variables. According to the most influential current hypothesis about its psychological significance [E. Donchin's], the “context updating” hypothesis, P3 reflects the updating of working memory. This hypothesis cannot account for relevant portions of the available evidence and it entails some basic contradictions. A more general formulation of this hypothesis is that P3 reflects the updating of expectancies. This version implies that P3-evoking stimuli (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • On the need for discipline in the construction of psychological theories.Donald Laming - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):669.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The scientific induction problem: A case for case studies.K. Anders Ericsson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):480-481.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionism and implementation.Paul Smolensky - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):492-493.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive self-organization and neural modularity.Stephen Grossberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):18-19.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resurrecting Lorenz's hydraulic model: Phlogiston explained by quantum mechanics.C. H. F. Rowell - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):397-398.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chaos, symbols, and connectionism.John A. Barnden - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):174-175.
    The paper is a commentary on the target article by Christine A. Skarda & Walter J. Freeman, “How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world”, in the same issue of the journal, pp.161–195. -/- I confine my comments largely to some philosophical claims that Skarda & Freeman make and to the relationship of their model to connectionism. Some of the comments hinge on what symbols are and how they might sit in neural systems.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The quantized geometry of visual space: The coherent computation of depth, form, and lightness.Stephen Grossberg - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):625.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • An operant analysis of problem solving.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):583-591.
    Behavior that solves a problem is distinguished by the fact that it changes another part of the solver's behavior and is strengthened when it does so. Problem solving typically involves the construction of discriminative stimuli. Verbal responses produce especially useful stimuli, because they affect other people. As a culture formulates maxims, laws, grammar, and science, its members behave more effectively without direct or prolonged contact with the contingencies thus formulated. The culture solves problems for its members, and does so by (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Functional principles and situated problem solving.William J. Clancey - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):479-480.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What constitutes a module?Peter W. Jusczyk & Asher Cohen - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):20-21.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • They are really complex when you get to know them.Irving Kupfermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):393-394.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognition as self–organizing process.Gerhard Werner - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):183-183.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Adaptive resonance theory: Problems with prediction.Mark Wagner - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):675.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Contingencies, rules, and the “problem” of novel behavior.Pere Julià - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):598-599.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Levels of research.Colleen Seifert & Donald A. Norman - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):490-492.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lexicon as module.Mark S. Seidenberg - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):31-32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Three frames suffice: Drop the retinotopic frame.Ralph Norman Haber - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):295-296.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A case study of how a paper containing good ideas, presented by a distinguished scientist, to an appropriate audience, had almost no influence at all.Earl Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):597-598.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Acquisition and extinction in autoshaping.Sham Kakade & Peter Dayan - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (3):533-544.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • We are making good progress in the neural analysis of behaviour.David L. Macmillan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):395-395.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Points of congruence between ethology and neuroscience.Wolfgang M. Schleidt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):398-399.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can neuroethologists be led?Fred Delcomyn - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):385.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the conceptual foundations of cognitive psychophysiology.Emanuel Donchin & Michael G. H. Coles - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):408.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning is critical, not implementation versus algorithm.James T. Townsend - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):497-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Quinity, isotropy, and Wagnerian rapture.Georges Rey - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):27-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A rapprochement of biology, psychology, and philosophy.Sandra Scarr - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):29-29.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function.Risto Näätänen - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):201-233.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • Stable self-organization of sensory recognition codes: Is chaos necessary?Stephen Grossberg - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):179-180.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can Skinner define a problem?Geir Kaufmann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):599-599.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionist models are also algorithmic.David S. Touretzky - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):496-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Methodologies for studying human knowledge.John R. Anderson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):467-477.
    The appropriate methodology for psychological research depends on whether one is studying mental algorithms or their implementation. Mental algorithms are abstract specifications of the steps taken by procedures that run in the mind. Implementational issues concern the speed and reliability of these procedures. The algorithmic level can be explored only by studying across-task variation. This contrasts with psychology's dominant methodology of looking for within-task generalities, which is appropriate only for studying implementational issues.The implementation-algorithm distinction is related to a number of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Does connectionism suffice?Steven W. Zucker - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):301-302.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Parallel processing explains modular informational encapsulation.Marcel Kinsbourne - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):23-23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Questions raised by the reinforcement paradigm.Anatol Rapoport - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):601-602.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural Dynamics of Autistic Repetitive Behaviors and Fragile X Syndrome: Basal Ganglia Movement Gating and mGluR-Modulated Adaptively Timed Learning.Stephen Grossberg & Devika Kishnan - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Methodological Problems on the Way to Integrative Human Neuroscience.Kotchoubey Boris, Tretter Felix, A. Braun Hans, Buchheim Thomas, Draguhn Andreas, Fuchs Thomas, Hasler Felix, Hastedt Heiner, Hinterberger Thilo, Northoff Georg, Rentschler Ingo, Schleim Stephan, Sellmaier Stephan, Van Elst Ludger Tebartz & Tschacher Wolfgang - unknown
    Neuroscience is a multidisciplinary effort to understand the structures and functions of the brain and brain-mind relations. This effort results in an increasing amount of data, generated by sophisticated technologies. However, these data enhance our descriptive knowledge, rather than improve our understanding of brain functions. This is caused by methodological gaps both within and between subdisciplines constituting neuroscience, and the atomistic approach that limits the study of macro- and mesoscopic issues. Whole-brain measurement technologies do not resolve these issues, but rather (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • ERPs and attention: Deep data, broad theory.Jeff Miller - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):249-250.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology: A call for less exclusivity and more theory.Michael A. Arbib - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):381.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chaos in brains: Fad or insight?Donald H. Perkel - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):180-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spatial analysis of brain function:Not the first.Robert M. Boynton - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):175-175.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Negation in Skinner's system.N. E. Wetherick - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):606-607.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolutionary aspect of cognitive functions.J. -P. Ewert - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):481-483.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On Spearman's “problem of correlation”.John B. Carroll - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):7-7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Variability, gnostic units and N2.Kristina T. Ciesielski - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):236-237.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark