Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis.Ashley de Marchena, Inge-Marie Eigsti, Amanda Worek, Kim Emiko Ono & Jesse Snedeker - 2011 - Cognition 119 (1):96-113.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Word frequency as a cue for identifying function words in infancy.Jean-Rémy Hochmann, Ansgar D. Endress & Jacques Mehler - 2010 - Cognition 115 (3):444-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Wordform variability in infants’ language environment and its effects on early word learning.Charlotte Moore & Elika Bergelson - 2024 - Cognition 245 (C):105694.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Should I learn from you? Seeing expectancy violations about action efficiency hinders social learning in infancy.Marc Colomer & Amanda Woodward - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105293.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are translation equivalents special? Evidence from simulations and empirical data from bilingual infants.Rachel Ka-Ying Tsui, Ana Maria Gonzalez-Barrero, Esther Schott & Krista Byers-Heinlein - 2022 - Cognition 225 (C):105084.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarity of referents influences the learning of phonological word forms: Evidence from concurrent word learning.Libo Zhao, Stephanie Packard, Bob McMurray & Prahlad Gupta - 2019 - Cognition 190 (C):42-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Bayesian Word Learning in Multiple Language Environments.Benjamin D. Zinszer, Sebi V. Rolotti, Fan Li & Ping Li - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):439-462.
    Infant language learners are faced with the difficult inductive problem of determining how new words map to novel or known objects in their environment. Bayesian inference models have been successful at using the sparse information available in natural child-directed speech to build candidate lexicons and infer speakers’ referential intentions. We begin by asking how a Bayesian model optimized for monolingual input generalizes to new monolingual or bilingual corpora and find that, especially in the case of the bilingual input, the model (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words.Susanne Grassmann, Cornelia Schulze & Michael Tomasello - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The Right Thing at the Right Time: Why Ostensive Naming Facilitates Word Learning.Emma L. Axelsson, Kirsten Churchley & Jessica S. Horst - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Object and word familiarization differentially boost retention in fast-mapping.Sarah C. Kucker & Larissa K. Samuelson - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: Evidence from autism.Melissa Allen Preissler & Susan Carey - 2005 - Cognition 97 (1):B13-B23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Exclusion Constraints Facilitate Statistical Word Learning.Katherine Yoshida, Mijke Rhemtulla & Athena Vouloumanos - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (5):933-947.
    The roles of linguistic, cognitive, and social-pragmatic processes in word learning are well established. If statistical mechanisms also contribute to word learning, they must interact with these processes; however, there exists little evidence for such mechanistic synergy. Adults use co-occurrence statistics to encode speech–object pairings with detailed sensitivity in stochastic learning environments (Vouloumanos, 2008). Here, we replicate this statistical work with nonspeech sounds and compare the results with the previous speech studies to examine whether exclusion constraints contribute equally to the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Role of Novelty in Early Word Learning.Emily Mather & Kim Plunkett - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (7):1157-1177.
    What mechanism implements the mutual exclusivity bias to map novel labels to objects without names? Prominent theoretical accounts of mutual exclusivity (e.g., Markman, 1989, 1990) propose that infants are guided by their knowledge of object names. However, the mutual exclusivity constraint could be implemented via monitoring of object novelty (see Merriman, Marazita, & Jarvis, 1995). We sought to discriminate between these contrasting explanations across two preferential looking experiments with 22-month-olds. In Experiment 1, infants viewed three objects: one name-known, two name-unknown. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • A One-to-One Bias and Fast Mapping Support Preschoolers' Learning About Faces and Voices.Mariko Moher, Lisa Feigenson & Justin Halberda - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (5):719-751.
    A multimodal person representation contains information about what a person looks like and what a person sounds like. However, little is known about how children form these face-voice mappings. Here, we explored the possibility that two cognitive tools that guide word learning, a one-to-one mapping bias and fast mapping, also guide children’s learning about faces and voices. We taught 4- and 5-year-olds mappings between three individual faces and voices, then presented them with new faces and voices. In Experiment 1, we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The role of developmental change and linguistic experience in the mutual exclusivity effect.Molly Lewis, Veronica Cristiano, Brenden M. Lake, Tammy Kwan & Michael C. Frank - 2020 - Cognition 198 (C):104191.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Too Much of a Good Thing: How Novelty Biases and Vocabulary Influence Known and Novel Referent Selection in 18‐Month‐Old Children and Associative Learning Models.Sarah C. Kucker, Bob McMurray & Larissa K. Samuelson - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):463-493.
    Identifying the referent of novel words is a complex process that young children do with relative ease. When given multiple objects along with a novel word, children select the most novel item, sometimes retaining the word‐referent link. Prior work is inconsistent, however, on the role of object novelty. Two experiments examine 18‐month‐old children's performance on referent selection and retention with novel and known words. The results reveal a pervasive novelty bias on referent selection with both known and novel names and, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Cross-situational learning in a Zipfian environment.Andrew T. Hendrickson & Amy Perfors - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):11-22.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Investigating the relationship between fast mapping, retention, and generalisation of words in children with autism spectrum disorder and typical development.Calum Hartley, Laura-Ashleigh Bird & Padraic Monaghan - 2019 - Cognition 187 (C):126-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Prediction error boosts retention of novel words in adults but not in children.Chiara Gambi, Martin J. Pickering & Hugh Rabagliati - 2021 - Cognition 211 (C):104650.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Statistical learning and memory.Ansgar D. Endress, Lauren K. Slone & Scott P. Johnson - 2020 - Cognition 204 (C):104346.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Advent and Fall of a Vocabulary Learning Bias from Communicative Efficiency.David Carrera-Casado & Ramon Ferrer-I.-Cancho - 2021 - Biosemiotics 14 (2):345-375.
    Biosemiosis is a process of choice-making between simultaneously alternative options. It is well-known that, when sufficiently young children encounter a new word, they tend to interpret it as pointing to a meaning that does not have a word yet in their lexicon rather than to a meaning that already has a word attached. In previous research, the strategy was shown to be optimal from an information theoretic standpoint. In that framework, interpretation is hypothesized to be driven by the minimization of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants.Krista Byers-Heinlein & Janet F. Werker - 2013 - Cognition 128 (3):407-416.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • How Native Prosody Affects Pitch Processing during Word Learning in Limburgian and Dutch Toddlers and Adults.Stefanie Ramachers, Susanne Brouwer & Paula Fikkert - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8:290015.
    In this study, Limburgian and Dutch 2.5- to 4-year-olds and adults took part in a word learning experiment. Following the procedure employed by Quam and Swingley (2010) and Singh et al. (2014), participants learned two novel word-object mappings. After training, word recognition was tested in correct pronunciation (CP) trials and mispronunciation (MP) trials featuring a pitch change. Since Limburgian is considered a restricted tone language, we expected that the pitch change would hinder word recognition in Limburgian, but not in non-tonal (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30months. [REVIEW]Ricardo Ah Bion, Arielle Borovsky & Anne Fernald - 2013 - Cognition 126 (1):39-53.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Mine, Mine, Mine: Self-Reference and Children’s Retention of Novel Words.Emma L. Axelsson, Rachelle L. Dawson, Sharon Y. Yim & Tashfia Quddus - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A distributional perspective on the gavagai problem in early word learning.Richard N. Aslin & Alice F. Wang - 2021 - Cognition 213 (C):104680.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mutual Exclusivity in Pragmatic Agents.Xenia Ohmer, Michael Franke & Peter König - 2021 - Cognitive Science 46 (1):e13069.
    One of the great challenges in word learning is that words are typically uttered in a context with many potential referents. Children's tendency to associate novel words with novel referents, which is taken to reflect a mutual exclusivity (ME) bias, forms a useful disambiguation mechanism. We study semantic learning in pragmatic agents—combining the Rational Speech Act model with gradient‐based learning—and explore the conditions under which such agents show an ME bias. This approach provides a framework for investigating a pragmatic account (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cross‐situational Learning From Ambiguous Egocentric Input Is a Continuous Process: Evidence Using the Human Simulation Paradigm.Yayun Zhang, Daniel Yurovsky & Chen Yu - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (7):e13010.
    Recent laboratory experiments have shown that both infant and adult learners can acquire word‐referent mappings using cross‐situational statistics. The vast majority of the work on this topic has used unfamiliar objects presented on neutral backgrounds as the visual contexts for word learning. However, these laboratory contexts are much different than the real‐world contexts in which learning occurs. Thus, the feasibility of generalizing cross‐situational learning beyond the laboratory is in question. Adapting the Human Simulation Paradigm, we conducted a series of experiments (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Interaction of phonological biases and frequency in learning a probabilistic language pattern.Hanbyul Song & James White - 2022 - Cognition 226 (C):105170.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Twelve-month-olds disambiguate new words using mutual-exclusivity inferences.Barbara Pomiechowska, Gábor Bródy, Gergely Csibra & Teodora Gliga - 2021 - Cognition 213 (C):104691.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cross‐Situational Word Learning With Multimodal Neural Networks.Wai Keen Vong & Brenden M. Lake - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (4).
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 4, April 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Spoken word recognition in early childhood: Comparative effects of vowel, consonant and lexical tone variation.Leher Singh, Hwee Hwee Goh & Thilanga D. Wewalaarachchi - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):1-11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • And then I saw her race: Race-based expectations affect infants’ word processing.Drew Weatherhead & Katherine S. White - 2018 - Cognition 177 (C):87-97.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neutralization and homophony avoidance in phonological learning.Sora Heng Yin & James White - 2018 - Cognition 179 (C):89-101.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations