Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Daddy, why are people so complex?Allan L. Combs - 2006 - World Futures 62 (6):464 – 472.
    The implications of Warren McCulloch's 1945 concept of heterarchy are analyzed in terms of human value and motivational systems. The results demonstrate the near-impossibility of predicting behavior on the basis of any hierarchical scheme, or even which among a set of hierarchical schemes will be selected as the basis of a behavioral choice. Thus, for example, people regularly say one thing and do another.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is anyone a cognitive ethologist?Colin Allen - 2004 - Biology and Philosophy 19 (4):589-607.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • On Seeing: Remarks on Metzger’s Laws of Seeing. [REVIEW]Liliana Albertazzi - 2011 - Axiomathes 21 (4):581-595.
    Nowadays cognitive science often views sensorial presentations and mental presentations as mutually exclusive, and they are also given separate treatment by neurophysiologists and by cognitive scientists, and some phenomena (like anomalous surfaces or various types of imagery) are reduced to either the former or the latter. Since no adequate methods for its investigation have been developed, the level of perceptual experiences analysed by Gestaltists and magnificently illustrated by Metzger in his Laws of Seeing remains unexplored. Starting from Metzger’s analyses the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Basic Emotions or Ur-Emotions?Nico H. Frijda & W. Gerrod Parrott - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (4):406-415.
    This article sets out to replace the concept of basic emotions with the notion of “ur-emotions,” the functionally central underlying processes of action readiness, which are not emotions at all. We propose that what is basic and universal in emotions are not multicomponential syndromes, but states of action readiness, themselves variants of motive states to relate or not relate with the world and with oneself. Unlike emotions, ur-emotions can be held to be universal and biologically based.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Epistemological discipline in animal behavior studies: Konrad Lorenz and Daniel Lehrman on intuition and empathy.Marga Vicedo - 2023 - History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 45 (1):1-32.
    Can empathy be a tool for obtaining scientific knowledge or is it incompatible with the detached objectivity that is often seen as the ideal in scientific inquiry? This paper examines the views of Austrian ethologist Konrad Lorenz and American comparative psychologist Daniel Lehrman on the role of intuition and empathy in the study of animal behavior. It situates those views within the larger project of establishing ethology as an objective science. Lehrman challenged Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen, the main founders of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What rationality adds to animal morality.Bruce N. Waller - 1997 - Biology and Philosophy 12 (3):341-356.
    Philosophical tradition demands rational reflection as a condition for genuine moral acts. But the grounds for that requirement are untenable, and when the requirement is dropped morality comes into clearer view as a naturally developing phenomenon that is not confined to human beings and does not require higher-level rational reflective processes. Rational consideration of rules and duties can enhance and extend moral behavior, but rationality is not necessary for morality and (contrary to the Kantian tradition represented by Thomas Nagel) morality (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Culture in whales and dolphins.Luke Rendell & Hal Whitehead - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (2):309-324.
    Studies of animal culture have not normally included a consideration of cetaceans. However, with several long-term field studies now maturing, this situation should change. Animal culture is generally studied by either investigating transmission mechanisms experimentally, or observing patterns of behavioural variation in wild populations that cannot be explained by either genetic or environmental factors. Taking this second, ethnographic, approach, there is good evidence for cultural transmission in several cetacean species. However, only the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops) has been shown experimentally to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • The ‘Disadapted’ Animal: Niko Tinbergen on Human Nature and the Human Predicament.Marga Vicedo - 2018 - Journal of the History of Biology 51 (2):191-221.
    This paper explores ethologist Niko Tinbergen’s path from animal to human studies in the 1960s and 1970s and his views about human nature. It argues, first, that the confluence of several factors explains why Tinbergen decided to cross the animal/human divide in the mid 1960s: his concern about what he called “the human predicament,” his relations with British child psychiatrist John Bowlby, the success of ethological explanations of human behavior, and his professional and personal situation. It also argues that Tinbergen (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Nature's Longing for Beauty: Elegance as an Evolutionary Attractor, with Implications for Human Systems.Charles Smith - 2010 - World Futures 66 (7):504-510.
    (2010). Nature's Longing for Beauty: Elegance as an Evolutionary Attractor, with Implications for Human Systems. World Futures: Vol. 66, No. 7, pp. 504-510.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What is a "Jewish Dog"? Konrad Lorenz and the Cult of Wildness.Boria Sax - 1997 - Society and Animals 5 (1):3-21.
    This paper explores the Nazi view of nature as violent but orderly, contrasted with what the Nazis took to be the chaos and confusion of human society. In imposing strict authoritarian controls, the Nazis strove to emulate what they viewed as the natural discipline of instinct. They saw this as embodied in wild animals, especially large predators such as wolves, while the opposite were domesticated mongrels whose instincts, like those of overly civilized peoples, had been ruined through careless breeding. Those (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Aesthetic cognition.Robert S. Root-Bernstein - 2002 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 16 (1):61 – 77.
    The purpose of this article is to integrate two outstanding problems within the philosophy of science. The first concerns what role aesthetics plays in scientific thinking. The second is the problem of how logically testable ideas are generated (the so-called "psychology of research" versus "logic of (dis)proof" problem). I argue that aesthetic sensibility is the basis for what scientists often call intuition, and that intuition in turn embodies (in a literal physiological sense) ways of thinking that have their own meta-logic. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Operant responding in the common crow.Robert W. Powell - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1 (6):401-403.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Nonhuman and Nonhuman-Human Communication: Some Issues and Questions.Irene M. Pepperberg - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Deciphering nonhuman communication – particularly nonhuman vocal communication – has been a longstanding human quest. We are, for example, fascinated by the songs of birds and whales, the grunts of apes, the barks of dogs, and the croaks of frogs; we wonder about their potential meaning and their relationship to human language. Do these utterances express little more than emotional states, or do they convey actual bits and bytes of concrete information? Humans’ numerous attempts to decipher nonhuman systems have, however, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Origins of Morality: An Essay in Philosophical Anthropology.Andrew Oldenquist - 1990 - Social Philosophy and Policy 8 (1):121.
    By what steps, historically, did morality emerge? Our remote ancestors evolved into social animals. Sociality requires, among other things, restraints on disruptive sexual, hostile, aggressive, vengeful, and acquisitive behavior. Since we are innately social and not social by convention, we can assume the biological evolution of the emotional equipment – numerous predispositions to want, fear, feel anxious or secure – required for social living, just as we can assume cultural evolution of various means to control antisocial behavior and reinforce the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Darwin and Darwin Studies, 1959–63.Bert James Loewenberg - 1965 - History of Science 4 (1):15.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Role of Attachment Trauma and Disintegrative Pathogenic Processes in the Traumatic-Dissociative Dimension.Benedetto Farina, Marianna Liotti & Claudio Imperatori - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Spinoza: Empowerment and the ethics of composition.Germán Ulises Bula Caraballo - 2012 - Universitas Philosophica 29 (58):197-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Not to harm a fly: our ethical obligations to insects.Jeffrey A. Lockwood - 1988 - Between the Species 4 (3):12.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations