Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Practical concepts and productive reasoning.Carlotta Pavese - 2021 - Synthese 199 (3-4):7659-7688.
    Can we think of a task in a distinctively practical way? Can there be practical concepts? In recent years, epistemologists, philosophers of mind, as well as philosophers of psychology have appealed to practical concepts in characterizing the content of know-how or in explaining certain features of skilled action. However, reasons for positing practical concepts are rarely discussed in a systematic fashion. This paper advances a novel argument for the psychological reality of practical concepts that relies on evidence for a distinctively (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The elephant in the China shop: When technical reasoning meets cumulative technological culture.François Osiurak & Emanuelle Reynaud - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43.
    The commentaries have both revealed the implications of and challenged our approach. In this response, we reply to these concerns, discuss why the technical-reasoning hypothesis does not minimize the role of social-learning mechanisms – nor assume that technical-reasoning skills make individuals omniscient technically – and make suggestions for overcoming the classical opposition between the cultural versus cognitive niche hypothesis of cumulative technological culture.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture.François Osiurak & Emanuelle Reynaud - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43:e156.
    Cumulative technological culture (CTC) refers to the increase in the efficiency and complexity of tools and techniques in human populations over generations. A fascinating question is to understand the cognitive origins of this phenomenon. Because CTC is definitely a social phenomenon, most accounts have suggested a series of cognitive mechanisms oriented toward the social dimension (e.g., teaching, imitation, theory of mind, and metacognition), thereby minimizing the technical dimension and the potential influence of non-social, cognitive skills. What if we have failed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Roles of Technical Reasoning, Theory of Mind, Creativity, and Fluid Cognition in Cumulative Technological Culture.Emmanuel De Oliveira, Emanuelle Reynaud & François Osiurak - 2019 - Human Nature 30 (3):326-340.
    Cumulative technological culture can be defined as the progressive diversification, complexification, and enhancement of technological traits through generations. An outstanding issue is to specify the cognitive bases of this phenomenon. Based on the literature, we identified four potential cognitive factors: namely, theory-of-mind, technical-reasoning, creativity, and fluid-cognitive skills. The goal of the present study was to test which of these factors—or a combination thereof—best predicted the cumulative performance in two experimental, micro-society conditions differing in the nature of the interaction allowed between (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Switching between lift and use grasp actions.Daniel N. Bub, Michael E. J. Masson & Hannah van Mook - 2018 - Cognition 174 (C):28-36.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Affordances, context and sociality.Anna M. Borghi - 2018 - Synthese 199 (5-6):12485-12515.
    Affordances, i.e. the opportunity of actions offered by the environment, are one of the central research topics for the theoretical perspectives that view cognition as emerging from the interaction between the environment and the body. Being at the bridge between perception and action, affordances help to question a dichotomous view of perception and action. While Gibson’s view of affordances is mainly externalist, many contemporary approaches define affordances as the product of long-term visuomotor associations in the brain. These studies have emphasized (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Representational Processes of Actions Toward and Away from the Body.Francesco Ruotolo, Gennaro Ruggiero, Teresa Pia Arabia, Laurent Ott, Yann Coello, Angela Bartolo & Tina Iachini - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (9):e13192.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 9, September 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neural Processes Underlying Tool Use in Humans, Macaques, and Corvids.María J. Cabrera-Álvarez & Nicola S. Clayton - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the Neurocognitive Co‐Evolution of Tool Behavior and Language: Insights from the Massive Redeployment Framework.François Osiurak, Caroline Crétel, Natalie Uomini, Chloé Bryche, Mathieu Lesourd & Emanuelle Reynaud - 2021 - Topics in Cognitive Science 13 (4):684-707.
    Understanding the link between brain evolution and the evolution of distinctive features of modern human cognition is a fundamental challenge. A still unresolved question concerns the co-evolution of tool behavior (i.e., tool use or tool making) and language. The shared neurocognitive processes hypothesis suggests that the emergence of the combinatorial component of language skills within the frontal lobe/Broca's area made possible the complexification of tool-making skills. The importance of the frontal lobe/Broca's area in tool behavior is somewhat surprising with regard (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How children come to (not) detect and apply multiple functions for objects: Rethinking perseveration and functional fixedness.Maria Pflüger, David Buttelmann & Birgit Elsner - 2024 - Cognition 251 (C):105902.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the Temporal Dynamics of Tool Use.François Osiurak, Giovanni Federico, Maria A. Brandimonte, Emanuelle Reynaud & Mathieu Lesourd - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mind the gap: a more evolutionarily plausible role for technical reasoning in cumulative technological culture.Ross Pain & Rachael L. Brown - 2020 - Synthese 199 (1-2):2467-2489.
    How do technologies that are too complex for any one individual to produce arise and persist in human populations? Contra prevailing views focusing on social learning, Osiurak and Reynaud argue that the primary driver for cumulative technological culture is our ability for technical reasoning. Whilst sympathetic to their overall position, we argue that two specific aspects of their account are implausible: first, that technical reasoning is unique to humans; and second, that technical reasoning is a necessary condition for the production (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Four ways of (mis-)conceiving embodiment in tool use.François Osiurak & Giovanni Federico - 2020 - Synthese 199 (1-2):3853-3879.
    A broader conception of the user’s perceptual, cognitive, and motor capabilities considers tools as body extensions. By identifying specific tool-related motor-grounded mechanisms, the embodied approach assumes that this “extensional phenomenon” takes place not only at a behavioral level but also at a psychological level. At least four ways of conceiving embodiment in tool use have been offered in relation to the concepts of incorporation, perception, knowledge, and observation. Nevertheless, the validity of these conceptions has been rarely, if not never, assessed. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How Our Cognition Shapes and Is Shaped by Technology: A Common Framework for Understanding Human Tool-Use Interactions in the Past, Present, and Future.François Osiurak, Jordan Navarro & Emanuelle Reynaud - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning, remembering, and predicting how to use tools: Distributed neurocognitive mechanisms: Comment on Osiurak and Badets (2016).Laurel J. Buxbaum - 2017 - Psychological Review 124 (3):346-360.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • What is the future for tool-specific generalized motor programs?François Osiurak - 2017 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 16 (4):701-708.
    A key issue in cognitive sciences is to understand the cognitive bases of human tool use. Answers have been provided by two competing approaches. The manipulation-based approach assumes that humans can use tools because of the ability to store sensorimotor knowledge about how to manipulate tools. By contrast, for the reasoning-based approach, human tool use is based on the ability to reason about physical object properties. Recently, Caruana and Cuccio proposed a kind of reconciliation, based on the distinction between three (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Response: Commentary: Effects of dividing attention on memory for declarative and procedural aspects of tool use.Shumita Roy & Norman W. Park - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do silhouettes and photographs produce fundamentally different object-based correspondence effects?Robert W. Proctor, Mei-Ching Lien & Lane Thompson - 2017 - Cognition 169 (C):91-101.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A Role for the Action Observation Network in Apraxia After Stroke.Gloria Pizzamiglio, Zuo Zhang, James Kolasinski, Jane M. Riddoch, Richard E. Passingham, Dante Mantini & Elisabeth Rounis - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Pedagogue, the Engineer, and the Friend.François Osiurak, Caroline Cretel, Naomi Duhau-Marmon, Isabelle Fournier, Lucie Marignier, Emmanuel De Oliveira, Jordan Navarro & Emanuelle Reynaud - 2020 - Human Nature 31 (4):462-482.
    Humans can follow different social learning strategies, sometimes oriented toward the models’ characteristics. The goal of the present study was to explore which who-strategy is preferentially followed in the technological context based on the models’ psychological characteristics. We identified three potential who-strategies: Copy the pedagogue, copy the engineer, and copy the friend. We developed a closed-group micro-society paradigm in which participants had to build the highest possible towers. Participants began with an individual building phase. Then, they were gathered to discuss (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Conscious awareness of motor fluidity improves performance and decreases cognitive effort in sequence learning.Stefano Ioannucci, Arnaud Boutin, Thomas Michelet, Alexandre Zenon & Arnaud Badets - 2021 - Consciousness and Cognition 95 (C):103220.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Alternative Object Use in Adults and Children: Embodied Cognitive Bases of Creativity.Alla Gubenko & Claude Houssemand - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Why does one need creativity? On a personal level, improvisation with available resources is needed for online coping with unforeseen environmental stimuli when existing knowledge and apparent action strategies do not work. On a cultural level, the exploitation of existing cultural means and norms for the deliberate production of novel and valuable artifacts is a basis for cultural and technological development and extension of human action possibilities across various domains. It is less clear, however, how creativity develops and how exactly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark