Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Predictability of meaning in grammatical encoding: Optional plural marking.Chigusa Kurumada & Scott Grimm - 2019 - Cognition 191 (C):103953.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The length of words reflects their conceptual complexity.Molly L. Lewis & Michael C. Frank - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):182-195.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Prosody leaks into the memories of words.Kevin Tang & Jason A. Shaw - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104601.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Changing word usage predicts changing word durations in New Zealand English.Márton Sóskuthy & Jennifer Hay - 2017 - Cognition 166 (C):298-313.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The role of constructions in understanding predictability measures and their correspondence to word duration.Joan Bybee & Earl Kjar Brown - forthcoming - Cognitive Linguistics.
    Studies of word predictability in context show that words in English tend to be shorter if they are predictable from the next word, and to a lesser extent, if they are predictable from the previous word. Some studies distinguish function and content words, but otherwise have not considered grammatical factors, treating all two-word sequences as comparable. Because function words are highly frequent, words occurring with them have low predictability. Highest predictability occurs within bigrams with two content words. Using the Buckeye (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chunking or predicting – frequency information and reduction in the perception of multi-word sequences.David Lorenz & David Tizón-Couto - 2019 - Cognitive Linguistics 30 (4):751-784.
    Journal Name: Cognitive Linguistics Issue: Ahead of print.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The emergence of word-internal repetition through iterated learning: Explaining the mismatch between learning biases and language design.Mitsuhiko Ota, Aitor San José & Kenny Smith - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104585.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Word Forms Reflect Trade‐Offs Between Speaker Effort and Robust Listener Recognition.Stephan C. Meylan & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (7):e13478.
    How do cognitive pressures shape the lexicons of natural languages? Here, we reframe George Kingsley Zipf's proposed “law of abbreviation” within a more general framework that relates it to cognitive pressures that affect speakers and listeners. In this new framework, speakers' drive to reduce effort (Zipf's proposal) is counteracted by the need for low‐frequency words to have word forms that are sufficiently distinctive to allow for accurate recognition by listeners. To support this framework, we replicate and extend recent work using (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Challenges of Large‐Scale, Web‐Based Language Datasets: Word Length and Predictability Revisited.Stephan C. Meylan & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (6):e12983.
    Language research has come to rely heavily on large‐scale, web‐based datasets. These datasets can present significant methodological challenges, requiring researchers to make a number of decisions about how they are collected, represented, and analyzed. These decisions often concern long‐standing challenges in corpus‐based language research, including determining what counts as a word, deciding which words should be analyzed, and matching sets of words across languages. We illustrate these challenges by revisiting “Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication” (Piantadosi, Tily, & Gibson, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Cross-Linguistic Trade-Offs and Causal Relationships Between Cues to Grammatical Subject and Object, and the Problem of Efficiency-Related Explanations.Natalia Levshina - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:648200.
    Cross-linguistic studies focus on inverse correlations (trade-offs) between linguistic variables that reflect different cues to linguistic meanings. For example, if a language has no case marking, it is likely to rely on word order as a cue for identification of grammatical roles. Such inverse correlations are interpreted as manifestations of language users’ tendency to use language efficiently. The present study argues that this interpretation is problematic. Linguistic variables, such as the presence of case, or flexibility of word order, are aggregate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A chimpanzee by any other name: The contributions of utterance context and information density on word choice.Cassandra L. Jacobs & Maryellen C. MacDonald - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105265.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sociophonetics: The Role of Words, the Role of Context, and the Role of Words in Context.Jennifer Hay - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 10 (4):696-706.
    This paper synthesizes a wide range of literature from sociolinguistics and cognitive psychology, to argue for a central role for the “word” as a vehicle of language variation and change. Three crucially interlinked strands of research are reviewed—the role of context in associative learning, the word-level storage of phonetic and contextual detail, and the phonetic consequences of skewed distributions of words across different contexts. I argue that the human capacity for associative learning, combined with attention to fine-phonetic detail at the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Not so fast: Fast speech correlates with lower lexical and structural information.Uriel Cohen Priva - 2017 - Cognition 160 (C):27-34.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations