Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Psychology in the 18th century: a view from encyclopaedias.Fernando Vidal - 1993 - History of the Human Sciences 6 (1):89-119.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Spooks and spoofs: relations between psychical research and academic psychology in Britain in the inter-war period.Elizabeth R. Valentine - 2012 - History of the Human Sciences 25 (2):67-90.
    This article describes the relations between academic psychology and psychical research in Britain during the inter-war period, in the context of the fluid boundaries between mainstream psychology and both psychical research and popular psychology. Specifically, the involvement with Harry Price of six senior academic psychologists: William McDougall, William Brown, J. C. Flugel, Cyril Burt, C. Alec Mace and Francis Aveling, is described. Personal, metaphysical and socio-historical factors in their collaboration are discussed. It is suggested that the main reason for their (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • 'Psychological man' and human subjectivity in historical perspective.Irmingard Staeuble - 1991 - History of the Human Sciences 4 (3):417-432.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The history of psychological categories.Roger Smith - 2005 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 36 (1):55-94.
    Psychological terms, such as ‘mind’, ‘memory’, ‘emotion’ and indeed ‘psychology’ itself, have a history. This history, I argue, supports the view that basic psychological categories refer to historical and social entities, and not to ‘natural kinds’. The case is argued through a wide ranging review of the historiography of western psychology, first, in connection with the field’s extreme modern diversity; second, in relation to the possible antecedents of the field in the early modern period; and lastly, through a brief introduction (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Engineering the Human Soul: Analyzing Psychological Expertise.Nikolas Rose - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):351-369.
    The ArgumentIn the liberal democratic capitalist societies of “the West,” psychological know-how has made itself indispensable, not only in the regulation of domains from the factory to the family but also in the ethical systems according to which citizens live their lives. We cannot fully understand the role that psychology has come to play in terms of the application of science, the diffusion of ideas, or the entrepreneurial activities of a profession. Rather, we need to see psychology as making possible (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • History and the history of the human sciences: what voice?Smith Roger - 1997 - History of the Human Sciences 10 (3):22-39.
    This paper discusses the historical voice in the history of the human sci ences. I address the question, 'Who speaks?', as a question about disci plinary identities and conventions of writing - identities and conventions which have the appearance of conditions of knowledge, in an area of activity where academic history and the history of science or intellectual history meet. If, as this paper contends, the subject-matter of the history of the human sciences is inherently contestable because of fundamental differences (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • 'To know our fellow men to do them good': American Psychology's enduring moral project.Graham Richards - 1995 - History of the Human Sciences 8 (3):1-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Self-Regard and Other-Regard: Reflexive Practices in American Psychology, 1890–1940.Jill G. Morawski - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):281-308.
    The ArgumentPsychology has been frequently subjected to the criticism that it is an unreflexive science — that it fails to acknowledge the reflexive properties of human action which influence psychologists themselves as well as their subjects. However, even avowedly unreflexive actions may involve reflexivity, and in this paper I suggest that the practices of psychology include reflexive ones. Psychology has an established tradition of silence about the self-awareness and sell-consciousness of its actors, whether those actors are experimenters, theorists, or participants (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The progress of introspection in America, 1896–1938.Kenton Kroker - 2003 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 34 (1):77-108.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The progress of introspection in America, 1896-1938.Kenton Kroker - 2003 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 34 (1):77-108.
    Most histories of psychology weave a story around the rise of objective methods of investigation and the decline of subjective introspection. This paper sidesteps such disciplinary stories by describing self-scrutiny as a practice that moved through a variety of cultural, social and technological contexts in early twentieth-century America. Edmund Jacobson's technique of 'progressive relaxation' is offered as a case in point. Jacobson, a Chicago clinician, developed this cure for nervousness out of his earlier research under E. B. Titchener, an experimental (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Ideology and science: The story of Polish psychology in the communist period.Leszek Koczanowicz & Iwona Koczanowicz-Dehnel - 2021 - History of the Human Sciences 34 (3-4):195-217.
    This article presents a fragment of the history of psychology in Poland, discussing its development in the years 1945–56, which saw sweeping political and geographical transformations. In that maelstrom of history, psychology was particularly affected by the effects of geopolitical changes, which led to its symbolic ‘arrest’ in 1952, when psychological practice was prohibited and all psychology courses were abolished at universities. Amnesty was declared only in 1956, with the demise of the so-called Stalinist ‘cult of personality’ and the onset (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychologists interpreting conversion: two American forerunners of the hermeneutics of suspicion.David Hay - 1999 - History of the Human Sciences 12 (1):55-72.
    Because of the importance of Puritanism in its history, one of the forms taken by religious Angst at the end of the 19th century in New England was uneasiness about the psychological nature and validity of the conversion experience. Apart from William James and G. Stanley Hall, the leading psychologists who investigated this phenomenon were Edwin Starbuck and James Leuba. Each had a different personal stance with regard to the plausibility of religious belief. In practice their differences of opinion over (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Histories of suspicion in a time of conspiracy: a reflection on Aubrey Lewis's history of paranoia.David J. Harper - 1994 - History of the Human Sciences 7 (3):89-109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive science meets the mark of the cognitive: putting the horse before the cart.Joe Gough - 2022 - Biology and Philosophy 38 (1):1-24.
    Among those living systems, which are cognizers? Among the behaviours of, and causes of behaviour in, living systems, which are cognitive? Such questions sit at the heart of a sophisticated, ongoing debate, of which the recent papers by Corcoran et al. ( 2020 ) and Sims and Kiverstein ( 2021 ) serve as excellent examples. I argue that despite their virtues, both papers suffer from flawed conceptions of the point of the debate. This leaves their proposals ill-motivated—good answers to the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • From natural disability to the moral man: Calvinism and the history of psychology.C. F. Goodey - 2001 - History of the Human Sciences 14 (3):1-29.
    Some humanist theologians within the French Reformed Church in the 17th century developed the notion that a disability of the intellect could exist in nature independently of any moral defect, freeing its possessors from any obligations of natural law. Sharpened by disputes with the church leadership, this notion began to suggest a species-type classification that threatened to override the importance of the boundary between elect and reprobate in the doctrine of predestination. This classification seems to look forward to the natural (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Introduction.Kurt Danziger - 1991 - History of the Human Sciences 4 (3):327-333.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Problems of powerlessness: psychological explanations of social inequality and civil unrest in post-war America.Karen Baistow - 2000 - History of the Human Sciences 13 (3):95-116.
    This article concerns the emergence of psychological constructs of personal power and control in the United States during the 1960s and 1970s and the ways in which they contributed to contemporary political explanations of social unrest. While social scientists and politicians at the time saw this unrest as a social problem that posed threats to social cohesion and stability, they located its cause not in the power structure of society but in the individual’s sense of his or her own powerlessness. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Historicizing Mind Science: Discourse, Practice, Subjectivity.Mitchell G. Ash - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):193-207.
    It is no longer necessary to defend current historiography of psychology against the strictures aimed at its early text book incarnations in the 1960s and 1970s. At that time, Robert Young and others denigrated then standard textbook histories of psychology for their amateurism and their justifications propaganda for specific standpoints in current psychology, disguised as history. Since then, at least some textbooks writers and working historians of psychology have made such criticisms their own. The demand for textbook histories continues nonetheless. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations