Switch to: References

Citations of:

Features of similarity

Psychological Review 84 (4):327-352 (1977)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Protocentrism will prevail: A reply to Krueger (2003), Mussweiler (2003), and Sedikides (2003).Rachel Karniol - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (3):595-600.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Egocentrism versus protocentrism: The status of self in social prediction.Rachel Karniol - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (3):564-580.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Similarity in linguistic categorization: The importance of necessary properties.Siva Kalyan - 2012 - Cognitive Linguistics 23 (3).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Hybrid Extensional Prototype Compositionality.Jussi Jylkkä - 2011 - Minds and Machines 21 (1):41-56.
    It has been argued that prototypes cannot compose, and that for this reason concepts cannot be prototypes (Osherson and Smith in Cognition 9:35–58, 1981; Fodor and Lepore in Cognition 58:253–270, 1996; Connolly et al. in Cognition 103:1–22, 2007). In this paper I examine the intensional and extensional approaches to prototype compositionality, arguing that neither succeeds in their present formulations. I then propose a hybrid extensional theory of prototype compositionality, according to which the extension of a complex concept is determined as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Why Fodor’s Theory of Concepts Fails.Jussi Jylkkä - 2009 - Minds and Machines 19 (1):25-46.
    Fodor’s theory of concepts holds that the psychological capacities, beliefs or intentions which determine how we use concepts do not determine reference. Instead, causal relations of a specific kind between properties and our dispositions to token a concept are claimed to do so. Fodor does admit that there needs to be some psychological mechanisms mediating the property–concept tokening relations, but argues that they are purely accidental for reference. In contrast, I argue that the actual mechanisms that sustain the reference determining (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Indirect scaling methods for testing quantitative emotion theories.Martin Junge & Rainer Reisenzein - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (7):1247-1275.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • A non-empiricist perspective on learning in layered networks.Michael I. Jordan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):497-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Categorization and affordances.Rebecca K. Jones & Anne D. Pick - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):292-293.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analyzing recognition and recall.Gregory V. Jones - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):242.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • ‘Species-typicality’: Can individuals have typical parts?Timothy D. Johnston - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):291-292.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Intuitions about mathematical beauty: A case study in the aesthetic experience of ideas.Samuel G. B. Johnson & Stefan Steinerberger - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):242-259.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Simplicity and Generalization: Short-cutting Abstraction in Children's Object Categorizations.Robert L. Goldstone Ji Y. Son, Linda B. Smith - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):626.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Learning words in space and time: Contrasting models of the suspicious coincidence effect.Gavin W. Jenkins, Larissa K. Samuelson, Will Penny & John P. Spencer - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104576.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Visuomotor experiments: Failure to replicate, or failure to match the theory?Marc Jeannerod - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):71-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Asymmetry Bias in Me, We–Others Distance Ratings. The Role of Social Stereotypes.Maria Jarymowicz, Marta Kamińska-Feldman & Anna Szuster - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The relational correspondence between category exemplars and names.Kimberly A. Jameson & Nancy Alvarado - 2003 - Philosophical Psychology 16 (1):25 – 49.
    While recognizing the theoretical importance of context, current research has treated naming as though semantic meaning were invariant and the same mapping of category exemplars and names should exist across experimental contexts. An assumed symmetry or bidirectionality in naming behavior has been implicit in the interchangeable use of tasks that ask subjects to match names to stimuli and tasks that ask subjects to match stimuli to names. Examples from the literature are discussed together with several studies of color naming and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Is good more alike than bad? Positive-negative asymmetry in the differentiation between options. A study on the evaluation of fictitious political profiles.Magdalena Jablonska, Andrzej Falkowski & Robert Mackiewicz - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Our research focuses on the perception of difference in the evaluations of positive and negative options. The literature provides evidence for two opposite effects: on the one hand, negative objects are said to be more differentiated, on the other, people are shown to see greater differences between positive options. In our study, we investigated the perception of difference between fictitious political candidates, hypothesizing greater differences among the evaluations of favorable candidates. Additionally, we analyzed how positive and negative information affect candidate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Context Matters: Recovering Human Semantic Structure from Machine Learning Analysis of Large‐Scale Text Corpora.Marius Cătălin Iordan, Tyler Giallanza, Cameron T. Ellis, Nicole M. Beckage & Jonathan D. Cohen - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13085.
    Applying machine learning algorithms to automatically infer relationships between concepts from large-scale collections of documents presents a unique opportunity to investigate at scale how human semantic knowledge is organized, how people use it to make fundamental judgments (“How similar are cats and bears?”), and how these judgments depend on the features that describe concepts (e.g., size, furriness). However, efforts to date have exhibited a substantial discrepancy between algorithm predictions and human empirical judgments. Here, we introduce a novel approach to generating (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Context Matters: Recovering Human Semantic Structure from Machine Learning Analysis of Large‐Scale Text Corpora.Marius Cătălin Iordan, Tyler Giallanza, Cameron T. Ellis, Nicole M. Beckage & Jonathan D. Cohen - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13085.
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 2, February 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Codes, relations, and mappings.J. Wesley Hutchinson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tags, alphabets, and the neglect of sound.Stewart H. Hulse - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):148-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Metaphysics and common usage.David L. Hull - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):290-291.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Cognitive principles for information management: The principles of mnemonic associative knowledge (P-MAK).Michael Huggett, Holger Hoos & Ronald A. Rensink - 2007 - Minds and Machines 17 (4):445-485.
    Information management systems improve the retention of information in large collections. As such they act as memory prostheses, implying an ideal basis in human memory models. Since humans process information by association, and situate it in the context of space and time, systems should maximize their effectiveness by mimicking these functions. Since human attentional capacity is limited, systems should scaffold cognitive efforts in a comprehensible manner. We propose the Principles of Mnemonic Associative Knowledge (P-MAK), which describes a framework for semantically (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analogical Comparison Promotes Theory‐of‐Mind Development.Christian Hoyos, William S. Horton, Nina K. Simms & Dedre Gentner - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (9):e12891.
    Theory‐of‐mind (ToM) is an integral part of social cognition, but how it develops remains a critical question. There is evidence that children can gain insight into ToM through experience, including language training and explanatory interactions. But this still leaves open the question of how children gain these insights—what processes drive this learning? We propose that analogical comparison is a key mechanism in the development of ToM. In Experiment 1, children were shown true‐ and false‐belief scenarios and prompted to engage in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The result model of precedent.John F. Horty - 2004 - Legal Theory 10 (1):19-31.
    The result model of precedent holds that a legal precedent controls a fortiori cases—those cases, that is, that are at least as strong for the winning side of the precedent as the precedent case itself. This paper defends the result model against some objections by Larry Alexander, drawing on ideas from the field of Artificial Intelligence and Law in order to define an appropriate strength ordering for cases.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Similarity is as similarity does? A critical inquiry into the effect of thematic association on similarity.Garrett Honke & Kenneth J. Kurtz - 2019 - Cognition 186 (C):115-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How and when does syntax perpetuate stereotypes? Probing the framing effects of subject-complement statements of equality.Kevin J. Holmes, Evan M. Doherty & Stephen J. Flusberg - 2022 - Thinking and Reasoning 28 (2):226-260.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Analogical Mapping by Constraint Satisfaction.Keith J. Holyoak & Paul Thagard - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (3):295-355.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   261 citations  
  • The scrambling theorem: A simple proof of the logical possibility of spectrum inversion.Donald D. Hoffman - 2006 - Consciousness and Cognition 15 (1):31-45.
    The possibility of spectrum inversion has been debated since it was raised by Locke and is still discussed because of its implications for functionalist theories of conscious experience . This paper provides a mathematical formulation of the question of spectrum inversion and proves that such inversions, and indeed bijective scramblings of color in general, are logically possible. Symmetries in the structure of color space are, for purposes of the proof, irrelevant. The proof entails that conscious experiences are not identical with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Transformation and alignment in similarity.Carl J. Hodgetts, Ulrike Hahn & Nick Chater - 2009 - Cognition 113 (1):62-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Is there curvature adaptation not attributable to purely intravisual phenomena?Julian Hochberg & Leon Festinger - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):71-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Factual memory?William Hirst - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):241.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model.Douglas L. Hintzman - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (4):411-428.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   209 citations  
  • Episodic versus semantic memory: A distinction whose time has come – and gone?Douglas L. Hintzman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):240.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Problems of paraphrase: Bottom’s dream.David Hills - 2007 - The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication 3.
    Philosophers and critics alike often contend that metaphors cannot or should not be paraphrased, ever. Yet a simple and decisive empirical argument — The Horse’s Mouth Argument—suffices to show that many metaphors can be paraphrased without violating the spirit in which they were put forward in the first place. This argument leaves us with urgent unanswered questions about the role of paraphrase in a more inclusive division of exegetical labor, about the tension between its notorious openendedness and its claim to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • A Quantitative Empirical Analysis of the Abstract/Concrete Distinction.Felix Hill, Anna Korhonen & Christian Bentz - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (1):162-177.
    This study presents original evidence that abstract and concrete concepts are organized and represented differently in the mind, based on analyses of thousands of concepts in publicly available data sets and computational resources. First, we show that abstract and concrete concepts have differing patterns of association with other concepts. Second, we test recent hypotheses that abstract concepts are organized according to association, whereas concrete concepts are organized according to (semantic) similarity. Third, we present evidence suggesting that concrete representations are more (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Familiarity, Priming, and Perception in Similarity Judgments.M. Hiatt Laura & J. Gregory Trafton - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (6):1450-1484.
    We present a novel way of accounting for similarity judgments. Our approach posits that similarity stems from three main sources—familiarity, priming, and inherent perceptual likeness. Here, we explore each of these constructs and demonstrate their individual and combined effectiveness in explaining similarity judgments. Using these three measures, our account of similarity explains ratings of simple, color-based perceptual stimuli that display asymmetry effects, as well as more complicated perceptual stimuli with structural properties; more traditional approaches to similarity solve one or the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Levels of stimulus control: A functional approach.R. J. Herrnstein - 1990 - Cognition 37 (1-2):133-166.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Can the brain be divided into a sensory and a motor part?Volker Henn - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):70-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • But what is the substance of connectionist representation?James Hendler - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):496-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Universals, particulars, and paradigms.Helen Heise - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):289-290.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Get rich quick: The signal to respond procedure reveals the time course of semantic richness effects during visual word recognition.Ian S. Hargreaves & Penny M. Pexman - 2014 - Cognition 131 (2):216-242.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Evaluating nonreplication: more theory and background necessary.Lewis O. Harvey - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):70-70.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Coordination when there are restricted and unrestricted options.Shaun P. Hargreaves Heap, David Rojo Arjona & Robert Sugden - 2017 - Theory and Decision 83 (1):107-129.
    One might expect that, in pure coordination games, coordination would become less frequent as the number of options increases. Contrary to this expectation, we report an experiment which found more frequent coordination when the option set was unrestricted than when it was restricted. To try to explain this result, we develop a method for eliciting the general rules that subjects use to identify salient options in restricted and unrestricted sets. We find that each such rule, if used by all subjects, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What connectionist models learn: Learning and representation in connectionist networks.Stephen José Hanson & David J. Burr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):471-489.
    Connectionist models provide a promising alternative to the traditional computational approach that has for several decades dominated cognitive science and artificial intelligence, although the nature of connectionist models and their relation to symbol processing remains controversial. Connectionist models can be characterized by three general computational features: distinct layers of interconnected units, recursive rules for updating the strengths of the connections during learning, and “simple” homogeneous computing elements. Using just these three features one can construct surprisingly elegant and powerful models of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   62 citations  
  • Transcending “transcending…”.Stephen Jośe Hanson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):656-657.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarity semantics and minimal changes of belief.Sven Ove Hansson - 1992 - Erkenntnis 37 (3):401-429.
    Different similarity relations on sets are introduced, and their logical properties are investigated. Close relationships are shown to hold between similarity relations that are based on symmetrical difference and operators of belief contraction that are based on relational selection functions. Two new rationality criteria for minimal belief contraction, the maximizing property and the reducing property, are proposed.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Learning and representation: Tensions at the interface.Steven José Hanson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):511-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The development of category learning strategies: What makes the difference?Rubi Hammer, Gil Diesendruck, Daphna Weinshall & Shaul Hochstein - 2009 - Cognition 112 (1):105-119.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Similarity-based categorization and fuzziness of natural categories.James A. Hampton - 1998 - Cognition 65 (2-3):137-165.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations