Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Epistemological discipline in animal behavior studies: Konrad Lorenz and Daniel Lehrman on intuition and empathy.Marga Vicedo - 2023 - History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 45 (1):1-32.
    Can empathy be a tool for obtaining scientific knowledge or is it incompatible with the detached objectivity that is often seen as the ideal in scientific inquiry? This paper examines the views of Austrian ethologist Konrad Lorenz and American comparative psychologist Daniel Lehrman on the role of intuition and empathy in the study of animal behavior. It situates those views within the larger project of establishing ethology as an objective science. Lehrman challenged Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen, the main founders of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The social nature of the mother's tie to her child: John Bowlby's theory of attachment in post-war America.Marga Vicedo - 2011 - British Journal for the History of Science 44 (3):401-426.
    This paper examines the development of British psychiatrist and psychoanalyst John Bowlby's views and their scientific and social reception in the United States during the 1950s. In a 1951 report for the World Health Organization Bowlby contended that the mother is the child's psychic organizer, as observational studies of children worldwide showed that absence of mother love had disastrous consequences for children's emotional health. By the end of the decade Bowlby had moved from observational studies of children in hospitals to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The ‘Disadapted’ Animal: Niko Tinbergen on Human Nature and the Human Predicament.Marga Vicedo - 2018 - Journal of the History of Biology 51 (2):191-221.
    This paper explores ethologist Niko Tinbergen’s path from animal to human studies in the 1960s and 1970s and his views about human nature. It argues, first, that the confluence of several factors explains why Tinbergen decided to cross the animal/human divide in the mid 1960s: his concern about what he called “the human predicament,” his relations with British child psychiatrist John Bowlby, the success of ethological explanations of human behavior, and his professional and personal situation. It also argues that Tinbergen (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • “It Felt More like a Revolution.” How Behavioral Ecology Succeeded Ethology, 1970–1990.Cora Stuhrmann - 2022 - Berichte Zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte 45 (1-2):135-163.
    Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, Volume 45, Issue 1-2, Page 135-163, June 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • “It Felt More like a Revolution.” How Behavioral Ecology Succeeded Ethology, 1970–1990.Cora Stuhrmann - 2022 - Berichte Zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte 45 (1-2):135-163.
    As soon as ethology's status diminished in the early 1970s, it was confronted with two successor disciplines, sociobiology and behavioral ecology. They were able to challenge ethology because it no longer provided markers of strong disciplinarity such as theoretical coherence, leading figures and a clear identity. While behavioral ecology developed organically out of the UK ethological research community into its own disciplinary standing, sociobiology presented itself as a US competitor to the ethological tradition. I will show how behavioral ecology took (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The con man as model organism: the methodological roots of Erving Goffman’s dramaturgical self.Michael Pettit - 2011 - History of the Human Sciences 24 (2):138-154.
    This article offers a historical analysis of the relationship between the practice of participant-observation among American sociologists and Erving Goffman’s dramaturgical model of the self. He was a social scientist who privileged ethnography in the field over the laboratory experiment, the survey questionnaire, or the mental test. His goal was a natural history of communication among humans. Rather than rely upon standardizing technologies for measurement, Goffman tried to obtain accurate recordings of human behavior through secretive observations. During the 1950s, he (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Ethologists in the Kindergarten: Natural Behavior, Social Rank, and the Search for the “Innate” in Early Human Ethology.Jakob Odenwald - 2022 - Berichte Zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte 45 (1-2):87-111.
    Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, Volume 45, Issue 1-2, Page 87-111, June 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Introduction: Emotion and the Sciences: Varieties of Empathy in Science, Art, and History.Susan Lanzoni - 2012 - Science in Context 25 (3):287-300.
    Emotion and feeling have only in the last decade become analytic concepts in the humanities, reflected in what some have called an “affective turn” in the academy at large. The study of emotion has also found a place in science studies and the history and philosophy of science, accompanied by the recognition that even the history of objectivity depends in a dialectical fashion on a history of subjectivity. This topical issue is a contribution to this larger trend across the humanities (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • “It Felt More like a Revolution.” How Behavioral Ecology Succeeded Ethology, 1970–1990.Sophia Gräfe & Cora Stuhrmann - 2022 - Berichte Zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte 45 (1-2):10-29.
    Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, Volume 45, Issue 1-2, Page 10-29, June 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark