Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Do Infants in the First Year of Life Expect Equal Resource Allocations?Melody Buyukozer Dawkins, Stephanie Sloane & Renée Baillargeon - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:417740.
    Recent research has provided converging evidence, using multiple tasks, of sensitivity to fairness in the second year of life. In contrast, findings in the first year have been mixed, leaving it unclear whether young infants possess an expectation of fairness. The present research examined the possibility that young infants might expect windfall resources to be divided equally between similar recipients, but might demonstrate this expectation only under very simple conditions. In three violation-of-expectation experiments, 9-month-olds (N = 120) expected an experimenter (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Perseverative responding in a violation-of-expectation task in 6.5-month-old infants.Andréa Aguiar & Renée Baillargeon - 2003 - Cognition 88 (3):277-316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Evolutionary precursors of social norms in chimpanzees: a new approach.Claudia Rudolf von Rohr, Judith M. Burkart & Carel P. van Schaik - 2011 - Biology and Philosophy 26 (1):1-30.
    Moral behaviour, based on social norms, is commonly regarded as a hallmark of humans. Hitherto, humans are perceived to be the only species possessing social norms and to engage in moral behaviour. There is anecdotal evidence suggesting their presence in chimpanzees, but systematic studies are lacking. Here, we examine the evolution of human social norms and their underlying psychological mechanisms. For this, we distinguish between conventions, cultural social norms and universal social norms. We aim at exploring whether chimpanzees possess evolutionary (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Young infants view physically possible support events as unexpected: New evidence for rule learning.Su-hua Wang, Yu Zhang & Renée Baillargeon - 2016 - Cognition 157 (C):100-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Detecting continuity violations in infancy: a new account and new evidence from covering and tube events.Su-hua Wang, Renée Baillargeon & Sarah Paterson - 2005 - Cognition 95 (2):129-173.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Chimpanzees’ Bystander Reactions to Infanticide.Claudia Rudolf von Rohr, Carel P. van Schaik, Alexandra Kissling & Judith M. Burkart - 2015 - Human Nature 26 (2):143-160.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • A model for discovering ‘containment’ relations.Shimon Ullman, Nimrod Dorfman & Daniel Harari - 2019 - Cognition 183 (C):67-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Infants anticipate probabilistic but not deterministic outcomes.Ernő Téglás & Luca L. Bonatti - 2016 - Cognition 157 (C):227-236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Another Look at Looking Time: Surprise as Rational Statistical Inference.Zi L. Sim & Fei Xu - 2019 - Topics in Cognitive Science 11 (1):154-163.
    Surprise—operationalized as looking time—has a long history in developmental research, providing a window into the perception and cognition of infants. Recently, however, a number of developmental researchers have considered infants’ and children's surprise in its own right. This article reviews empirical evidence and computational models of complex statistical inferences underlying surprise, and discusses how these findings relate to the role that surprise appears to play as a catalyst for learning.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Young infants' expectations about hidden objects.Ted Ruffman, Lance Slade & Jessica Redman - 2005 - Cognition 97 (2):B35-B43.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Primate social knowledge and the origins of language.Robert M. Seyfarth & Dorothy L. Cheney - 2008 - Mind and Society 7 (1):129-142.
    Primate vocal communication is very different from human language. Differences are most pronounced in call production. Differences in production have been overemphasized, however, and distracted attention from the information that primates acquire when they hear vocalizations. In perception and cognition, continuities with language are more apparent. We suggest that natural selection has favored nonhuman primates who, upon hearing vocalizations, form mental representations of other individuals, their relationships, and their motives. This social knowledge constitutes a discrete, combinatorial system that shares several (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Understanding the abstract role of speech in communication at 12months.Alia Martin, Kristine H. Onishi & Athena Vouloumanos - 2012 - Cognition 123 (1):50-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants.Yuyan Luo & Renée Baillargeon - 2005 - Cognition 95 (3):297-328.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants.Yuyan Luo & Renée Baillargeon - 2005 - Cognition 95 (3):297-328.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Intuitions about magic track the development of intuitive physics.Casey Lewry, Kaley Curtis, Nadya Vasilyeva, Fei Xu & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2021 - Cognition 214 (C):104762.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Containment and Support: Core and Complexity in Spatial Language Learning.Barbara Landau, Kristen Johannes, Dimitrios Skordos & Anna Papafragou - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S4):748-779.
    Containment and support have traditionally been assumed to represent universal conceptual foundations for spatial terms. This assumption can be challenged, however: English in and on are applied across a surprisingly broad range of exemplars, and comparable terms in other languages show significant variation in their application. We propose that the broad domains of both containment and support have internal structure that reflects different subtypes, that this structure is reflected in basic spatial term usage across languages, and that it constrains children's (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Young infants’ actions reveal their developing knowledge of support variables: Converging evidence for violation-of-expectation findings.Susan J. Hespos & Renée Baillargeon - 2008 - Cognition 107 (1):304-316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Occlusion Is Hard: Comparing Predictive Reaching for Visible and Hidden Objects in Infants and Adults.Susan Hespos, Gustaf Gredebäck, Claes Von Hofsten & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (8):1483-1502.
    Infants can anticipate the future location of a moving object and execute a predictive reach to intercept the object. When a moving object is temporarily hidden by darkness or occlusion, 6‐month‐old infants’ reaching is perturbed, but performance on darkness trials is significantly better than occlusion trials. How does this reaching behavior change over development? Experiment 1 tested predictive reaching of 6‐ and 9‐month‐old infants. While there was an increase in the overall number of reaches with increasing age, there were significantly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations