Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Grünbaum's philosophical critique of psychoanalysis: Or what I don't know isn't knowledge.Paul Kline - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):245-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The scientific tasks confronting psychoanalysis.Gerald L. Klerman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):245-245.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The trouble with human sociobiology is ….Philip Kitcher - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):201-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolution of intelligence: making assumptions explicit and hypotheses testable.J. Kitahara-Frisch - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):390-391.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Précis of Vaulting Ambition: Sociobiology and the Quest for Human Nature.Philip Kitcher - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):61-71.
    The debate about the credentials of sociobiology has persisted because scholars have failed to distinguish the varieties of sociobiology and because too little attention has been paid to the details of the arguments that are supposed to support the provocative claims about human social behavior. I seek to remedy both deficiencies. After analysis of the relationships among different kinds of sociobiology and contemporary evolutionary theory, I attempt to show how some of the studies of the behavior of nonhuman animals meet (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • Confessions of a curmudgeon.Philip Kitcher - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):89-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Śūnyatā and kokoro: Science–religion dialogue in the japanese context.Seung Chul Kim - 2015 - Zygon 50 (1):155-171.
    When we read books or essays about the dialogue between “religion and science,” or when we attend conferences on the theme of “religion and science,” we cannot avoid the impression that they actually are dealing, almost without exception, not with a dialogue between “religion and science,” but with a dialogue between “Christianity and science.” This could easily be affirmed by looking at the major publications in this field. But how can the science–religion dialogue take place in a world where conventional (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Time to integrate sociobiology and social psychology.Douglas T. Kenrick & Richard C. Keefe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):24-26.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Validating psychoanalysis: what methods for what task?Horst Kächele - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):244-245.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Circle of Inclusion: Sustainability, CSR and the Values that Drive Them.Kerul Kassel - 2012 - Journal of Human Values 18 (2):133-146.
    This article examines the values that underlie notions of sustainability and corporate social responsibility, and how those values influence perspectives on who and what is included in decision-making. The author argues that the epistemologies of economic considerations in industrial practices, and how they are framed by science and technology, make it unlikely that practices towards sustainability will successfully avert expanding global crises unless the circle of inclusion is expanded to include other-than-human life.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Sexual strategies and social-class differences in fitness in modern industrial societies.Hillard Kaplan & Kim Hill - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):198-201.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • The trouble with terror.Joseph Margolis - 2007 - Metaphilosophy 38 (5):551-577.
    The argument proceeds from a sense of imminent danger; 9/11 and its sequel challenge our deepest pretensions regarding the universality and self-evidence of moral/political conviction. The intransigence of such convictions is now an important source of international conflict and terror. It also signifies that the resolution of the disorder that now confronts the international community requires a transformation in our conception of morality itself. In this regard, philosophy has an important task to address. The discussion explores a radical change in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Varieties of group selection.Doug Jones - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):778-779.
    Group selection may be defined either broadly or narrowly. Narrowly defined group selection may involve either selection for altruism or group selection between alternative evolutionarily stable states. The last variety of group selection is likely to have been particularly important in human evolution.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The ‘Biophilic Organization’: An Integrative Metaphor for Corporate Sustainability.David R. Jones - 2016 - Journal of Business Ethics 138 (3):401-416.
    This paper proposes a new organizational metaphor, the ‘Biophilic Organization’, which aims to counter the bio-cultural disconnection of many organizations despite their espoused commitment to sustainability. This conceptual research draws on multiple disciplines such as evolutionary psychology and architecture to not only develop a diverse bio-cultural connection but to show how this connection tackles sustainability, in a holistic and systemic sense. Moreover, the paper takes an integrative view of sustainability, which effectively means that it embraces the different emergent tensions. Three (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Social Darwinism revisited.Greta Jones - 1994 - History of European Ideas 19 (4-6):769-775.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Feeding versus social factors in cognitive evolution: can't we have it both ways?Alison Jolly - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):389-390.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Epigenesis and phylogenesis: Re-ordering the priorities.Timothy D. Johnston & Gilbert Gottlieb - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):243-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Concepts of development in the mathematics of cultural change.Timothy D. Johnston - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):14-15.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Amplifying sociobiology's hollow ring.Timothy D. Johnston - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):78-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Medical Futility and the Death of a Child.Nancy S. Jecker - 2011 - Journal of Bioethical Inquiry 8 (2):133-139.
    Our response to death may differ depending on the patient’s age. We may feel that death is a sad, but acceptable event in an elderly patient, yet feel that death in a very young patient is somehow unfair. This paper explores whether there is any ethical basis for our different responses. It examines in particular whether a patient’s age should be relevant to the determination that an intervention is medically futile. It also considers the responsibilities of health professionals and the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The pursuit of happiness: The social and scientific origins of Hans Selye’s natural philosophy of life.Mark Jackson - 2012 - History of the Human Sciences 25 (5):13-29.
    In 1956, Hans Selye tentatively suggested that the scientific study of stress could ‘help us to formulate a precise program of conduct’ and ‘teach us the wisdom to live a rich and meaningful life’. Nearly two decades later, Selye expanded this limited vision of social order into a full-blown philosophy of life. In Stress without Distress, first published in 1974, he proposed an ethical code of conduct designed to mitigate personal and social problems. Basing his arguments on contemporary understandings of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Human ethology and the ontogeny of emotional expressions.Carroll E. Izard - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):39-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the development of sign systems in primates.V. V. Ivanov - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):388-389.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolutionary hypotheses.Glynn L. Isaac - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):388-388.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How did morality evolve?William Irons - 1991 - Zygon 26 (1):49-89.
    This paper presents and criticizes. Alexander's evolutionary theory of morality (1987). Earlier research, on which Alexander's theory is based, is also reviewed. The propensity to create moral systems evolved because it allowed ancestral humans to limit conflict within cooperating groups and thus form larger groups, which were advantageous because of intense between-group competition. Alexander sees moral codes as contractual, and the primary criticism of his theory is that moral codes are not completely contractual but also coercive. Ways of evaluating Alexander's (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   86 citations  
  • Incest avoidance: shall we drop the genetic leash?William Irons - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):108-109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mating preferences surveys: Ethnographic follow-up would be a good next step.William Irons - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):24-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Social and reproductive success: Useful data but rethink the theory.William Irons - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):197-198.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Universality and species specificity.David L. Hull - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):38-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The essence of sociobiology.David L. Hull - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):242-243.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dialectics and evolution.Herbert Hörz - 1987 - Biology and Philosophy 2 (4):493-508.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Gene talk in sociobiology.Henry Howe & John Lyne - 1992 - Social Epistemology 6 (2):109-163.
    Terminology within the biological sciences gets its import not just from semantic meaning, but also from the way it functions within the rhetorics of the various disciplinary practices. The ‘sociobiology’ of human behavior inherits three distinct rhetorics from the genetic disciplines. Sociobiologists use population genetic, biometrical genetic, and molecular genetic rhetorics, without acknowledging the conceptual and experimental constraints that are assumed by geneticists. The eclectic blending of these three rhetorics obscures important differences of context and meaning. Sociobiologists use foundational terms (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Person schemas: Evolutionary, individual developmental and social sources.Mardi J. Horowitz - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):309-310.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some reflections on testing psychoanalytic hypotheses.Robert R. Holt - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):242-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Criticism, commitment, and the growth of human sociobiology.Harmon R. Holcomb - 1987 - Biology and Philosophy 2 (1):43-63.
    The fundamental unit of assessment in the sociobiology debate is neither a field nor a theory, but a framework of group commitments. Recourse to the framework concept is motivated, in general, by post-Kuhnian philosophy of scientific change and, in particular, by the dispute between E. O. Wilson and R. C. Lewontin. The framework concept is explicated in terms of commitments about problems, domain, disciplinary relations, exemplars, and performance evaluations. One upshot is that debate over such charges as genetic determinism, reductionism, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The problem of human ethology from the perspective of an experimental psychologist.Howard S. Hoffman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):37-38.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Repressed infantile wishes as the instigators of all dreams.J. Allan Hobson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):241-242.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Success in a dual evolutionary model.J. Hill - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):196-197.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some complexities in the evolution of language.Gordon W. Hewes - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):387-388.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The evolutionary epic.Philip Hefner - 2009 - Zygon 44 (1):3-8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Sociobiology, ethics, and theology.Philip Hefner - 1984 - Zygon 19 (2):185-207.
    The topic of sociobiology and ethics opens up a range of questions that have to do with important relationships: between the history of nature and human being, between biological evolution and psychosocial evolution, between is and ought, between language usages in one domain and another. The task of ethics is properly to discern what sociobiology has to tell us about the fundamentals of life and persuasively to direct our actions in accord with those fundamentals, in a manner that is consistent (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • An eclectric history of ethological theory and methods.Glenn Hausfater - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):36-37.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Too many P's in the pod.John Hartung - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):23-23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The biological perspective on suicide: to be or not to be - is that sociobiology?Morton G. Harmatz - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):280-281.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sexual attraction: A test case of sociobiological theory.H. V. C. Harris - 1984 - Zygon 19 (3):317-330.
    A study of the place of human sexuality in religious systems indicates a possible universal stress on sexual attraction. This could be explained by using the theories of Richard Dawkins and other sociobiologists: the philandering male and the coy female express the best strategies for the survival of the “selfish gene.” Closer analysis of four religious systems throws doubt on these theories. In some systems the strategies are contradicted while in others there is stress on cooperative restraint rather than on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Proximate mechanisms and distal objectives.John Hartung - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):196-196.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • Locus of causation: analysis of ethological similarities.Gordon M. Harrington - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):625-626.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Faulting ambition: A double standard?Henry Harpending - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (1):78-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biobehavioural basis of art.R. F. Harle - 2009 - Technoetic Arts 6 (3):259-268.
    This paper argues that the human activity of art making and art usage has a biological foundation which precedes language acquisition. Together with cultural dynamics we have created ourselves as we have created our arts. I attempt a synthesis of the theories of Dissanayake and Joyce which shows the human trait of art behaviour evolved as surely as did the behaviours of mating and hunting.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A too simple view of population genetics.Daniel L. Hartl - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):13-14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark