Switch to: References

Citations of:

On Human Nature

Harvard University Press (1978)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Mechanisms matter: The difference between socioblology and evolutionary psychology.Linnda R. Caporael - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):17-18.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Culture analyzed in the mode of the natural sciences.Edward O. Wilson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):116-117.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is van den Berghe in a new paradigm?Michael Ruse - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):113-114.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A coup de grace to cultural relativism.Joseph Shepher - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):114-114.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stability and variation in human evolution.Lionel Tiger - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):115-116.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some questions on optimal inbreeding and biologically adaptive culture.George C. Williams - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):116-116.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do humans maximize their inclusive fitness?Frank B. Livingstone - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):110-111.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The incestuous mind.Charles J. Lumsden - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):112-112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A psychologist's perspective on incest avoidance behavior.Karin C. Meiselman - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):112-112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inbreeding, cousin marriage, and social solidarity.Umberto Melotti - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):112-113.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A case for less selfing and more outbreeding in reviewing the literature.Michael E. Lamb & Eric L. Charnov - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):109-109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sexual rivalry in human inbreeding or adaptive cooperation?Chet S. Lancaster - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):109-110.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preculture versus culture?Daniel G. Freedman - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):107-108.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Incest avoidance: shall we drop the genetic leash?William Irons - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):108-109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Uncritical periods and insensitive sociobiology.Patrick Bateson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):102-103.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The intensity of human inbreeding depression.A. H. Bittles - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):103-104.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hypotheses are like people — some fit, some unfit.Ray H. Bixler - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):104-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Explaining inbreeding avoidance requires more complex models.Martin Daly & Margo Wilson - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):105-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Opportunity costs of inbreeding.Richard Dawkins - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):105-106.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Human inbreeding avoidance: Culture in nature.Pierre L. van den Berghe - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):91-102.
    Much clinical and ethnographic evidence suggests that humans, like many other organisms, are selected to avoid close inbreeding because of the fitness costs of inbreeding depression. The proximate mechanism of human inbreeding avoidance seems to be precultural, and to involve the interaction of genetic predispositions and environmental conditions. As first suggested by E. Westermarck, and supported by evidence from Israeli kibbutzim, Chinese sim-pua marriage, and much convergent ethnographic and clinical evidence, humans negatively imprint on intimate associates during a critical period (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   153 citations  
  • Similarity and ethnicity mediate human relationships, but why?J. Philippe Rushton - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):548-559.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Group selection and “the pious gene”.John Barresi - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):777-778.
    If selection at the group level is to be considered more than a mere possibility, it is important to find phenomena that are best explained at this level of selection. I argue that human religious phenomena provide evidence for the selection of a “pious gene” at the group level, which results in a human tendency to believe in a transcendental reality that encourages behavioral conformity to collective as opposed to individual interest.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Varieties of group selection.Doug Jones - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):778-779.
    Group selection may be defined either broadly or narrowly. Narrowly defined group selection may involve either selection for altruism or group selection between alternative evolutionarily stable states. The last variety of group selection is likely to have been particularly important in human evolution.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is selected in group selection?Michael E. Lamb - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):779-779.
    Misunderstandings often develop when scientists from different backgrounds use the same words (e.g., “selection”) when they mean different things by them. Theorists must therefore choose and define their terms carefully. In addition, proponents of “new” theories need to demonstrate empirically that theirs are more powerful than the existing theories they wish to supplant. Wilson & Sober have not yet done this.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Group selection or categorical perception?Craig T. Palmer, B. Eric Fredrickson & Christopher F. Tilley - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):780-780.
    Humans appear to be possible candidates for group selection because they are often said to live in bands, clans, and tribes. These terms, however, are only names for conceptual categories of people. They do not designate enduring bounded gatherings of people that might be “vehicles of selection.” Hence, group selection has probably not been a major force in human evolution.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Authoritarianism as a group-level adaptation in humans.Sven van de Wetering - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):780-781.
    Wilson & Sober's discussion of group selection is marred by the absence of plausible examples of human group-level behavioral adaptation. The trait of authoritarianism is one possible example of such an adaptation. It reduces within-group variance in reproductive success, manifests itself more strongly in response to group-level threat, and is found in a variety of cultures.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Genier than thou.Mike Waller - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):781-782.
    Many neo-Darwinists treat natural selection of genes and individual organisms as broadly equivalent. This enables Wilson & Sober (W&S) to propose a multilevel group selection model by drawing parallels between individuals and groups. The notion of gene/individual equivalence is a profound misconception. Its elimination negates W&S's current approach but offers the best way forward for both life and behavioural sciences.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • More on group selection and human behavior.David Sloan Wilson & Elliott Sober - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):782-787.
    The six commentaries raise five issues about multi-level selection theory that we attempt to address: (1) replicators without vehicles, (2) group selection and movement between groups, (3) absolute versus relative fitness, (4) group-level psychological adaptions, and (5) multi-level selection as a predictive theory.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Vormt moderne antropologie een probleem voor het Christelijk geloof?Luco J. Van den Brom - 2013 - HTS Theological Studies 69 (1):01-10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The One Per Cent Advantage: The Sociobiology of Being Human. By J. & M. Gribbin. Pp. 224. (Basil Blackwell, Oxford, 1988.) £12.95. [REVIEW]J. Lazarus - 1990 - Journal of Biosocial Science 22 (2):265-267.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Rebirth of Kinship.Mary K. Shenk & Siobhán M. Mattison - 2011 - Human Nature 22 (1-2):1-15.
    Kinship was one of the key areas of research interest among anthropologists in the nineteenth century, one of the most hotly debated areas of theory in the early and mid-twentieth century, and yet an area of waning interest by the end of the twentieth century. Since then, the study of kinship has experienced a revitalization, with concomitant disputes over how best to proceed. This special issue brings together recent studies of kinship by scientific anthropologists employing evolutionary theory and quantitative methods. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Reply to dickemann.Jay R. Feierman - 1992 - Human Nature 3 (3):279-297.
    This paper is a response to Dickemann’s review ofPedophilia: Biosocial Dimensions. Her main criticism of the book is its inappropriate application of ethology to human sexology and its natural variations. She proposes instead the superiority of the “social constructionist” perspective. The “Phylogenetic Fallacy” of which her review speaks results from her erroneously having attributed ethological arguments about the phylogeny of coordinated motor patterns and sensory releasing stimuli to higher levels of behavioral-ecological strategies to which such arguments were never applied. Because (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rules as the Impetus of Cultural Evolution.Jaroslav Peregrin - 2014 - Topoi 33 (2):531-545.
    In this paper I put forward a thesis regarding the anatomy of “cultural evolution”, in particular the way the “cultural” transmission of behavioral patterns came to piggyback, through us humans, on the transmission effected by genetic evolution. I claim that what grounds and supports this new kind of transmission is a complex behavioral “meta-pattern” that makes it possible to grasp a pattern as something that “ought to be”, i.e. that transforms the pattern into what we can call a rule. (Here (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Engineering Human Cooperation.Terence C. Burnham & Brian Hare - 2007 - Human Nature 18 (2):88-108.
    In a laboratory experiment, we use a public goods game to examine the hypothesis that human subjects use an involuntary eye-detector mechanism for evaluating the level of privacy. Half of our subjects are “watched” by images of a robot presented on their computer screen. The robot—named Kismet and invented at MIT—is constructed from objects that are obviously not human with the exception of its eyes. In our experiment, Kismet produces a significant difference in behavior that is not consistent with existing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Evolution and literary theory.Joseph Carroll - 1995 - Human Nature 6 (2):119-134.
    Presupposing that all knowledge is the study of a unitary order of nature, the author maintains that the study of literature should be included within the larger field of evolutionary theory. He outlines four elementary concepts in evolutionary theory, and he argues that these concepts should regulate our understanding of literature. On the basis of these concepts, he repudiates the antirealist and irrationalist views that, under the aegis of “poststructuralism,” have dominated academic literary studies for the past two decades. He (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Fuller's project of humanity: social sciences or sociobiology?: Steve Fuller, The New Sociological Imagination. London: Sage Publications, 2006.Francis Remedios - 2009 - History of the Human Sciences 22 (2):115-120.
    Review of Fuller’s New Sociological Imagination.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From behavior to culture: An assessment of cultural evolution and a new synthesis.Dwight Read - 2003 - Complexity 8 (6):17-41.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Genes beget memes and memes beget genes: Modeling a new catalytic closure.James N. Gardner - 1999 - Complexity 4 (5):22-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The logical relation between cultural and biological evolution: On to the next question.Jerome H. Barkow - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):235-236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some problems with an “options” view of evolution.Douglas Lee Eckberg - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):241-242.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Criticizing sociobiology: It's all been said before.Peter H. Klopfer - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (2):244-244.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dissecting the Neural Mechanisms Mediating Empathy.Jean Decety - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (1):92-108.
    Empathy is thought to play a key role in motivating prosocial behavior, guiding our preferences and behavioral responses, and providing the affective and motivational base for moral development. While these abilities have traditionally been examined using behavioral methods, recent work in evolutionary biology, developmental and cognitive neuroscience has begun to shed light on the neural circuitry that instantiate them. The purpose of this article is to critically examine the current knowledge in the field of affective neuroscience and provide an integrative (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Introduction.Dario Martinelli - 2009 - Sign Systems Studies 37 (3/4):353-368.
    Realism has been a central object of attention among analytical philosophers for some decades. Starting from analytical philosophy, the return of realism has spread into other contemporary philosophical traditions and given birth to new trends in current discussions, as for example in the debates about “new realism.” Discussions about realism focused on linguistic meaning, epistemology, metaphysics, theory of action and ethics. The implications for politics of discussion about realism in action theory and in ethics, however, are not much discussed.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Mistakes and Mental Disturbances: Pleasants, Wittgenstein, and Basic Moral Certainty.Robert Greenleaf Brice - 2013 - Philosophia 41 (2):477-487.
    In his article, “Wittgenstein and Basic Moral Certainty,” Nigel Pleasants argues that killing an innocent, non-threatening person is wrong. It is, he argues, “a basic moral certainty.” He believes our basic moral certainties play the “same kind of foundational role as [our] basic empirical certaint[ies] do.” I believe this is mistaken. There is not “simply one kind of foundational role” that certainty plays. While I think Pleasants is right to affiliate his proposition with a Wittgensteinian form of certainty, he exposes (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Antiaesthetics. [REVIEW]Allen Carlson - 1987 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 17 (4):919-933.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sociobiology. [REVIEW]Paul Thagard - 1981 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 11 (4):751-759.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On Moral Enhancement from a Habermasian Perspective.Hans-Joerg Ehni & Diana Aurenque - 2012 - Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics 21 (2):223-234.
    The human being’s mastery of itself, on which the self is founded, practically always involves the annihilation of the subject in whose service that mastery is maintained, because the substance which is mastered, suppressed, and disintegrated by self-preservation is nothing other than the living entity.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Feminist Engagement with Evolutionary Psychology.Carla Fehr - 2012 - Hypatia 27 (1):50-72.
    In this paper, I ask feminist philosophers and science studies scholars to consider the goals of developing critical analyses of evolutionary psychology. These goals can include development of scholarship in feminist philosophy and science studies, mediation of the uptake of evolutionary psychology by other academic and lay communities, and improvement of the practices and products of evolutionary psychology itself. I evaluate ways that some practices of feminist philosophy and science studies facilitate or hinder meeting these goals, and consider the merits (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Review: Sociobiology: Twenty-Five Years Later. [REVIEW]Michael Yudell & Rob Desalle - 2000 - Journal of the History of Biology 33 (3):577 - 584.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bashing the Enlightenment: A Discussion of Charles Taylor's Sources of the Self.Ronald de Sousa - 1994 - Dialogue 33 (1):109.
    This is a Big Book from one of Canada's preeminent philosophers. It aims at nothing less than to define what characterizes modernity, and then to tell us what is wrong with it. Like many a Big Book, it is predictably full of interesting things, and equally predictably disappointing, not to say feeble, in some of the central theses for which it argues. But then what more, in philosophy, can we really expect? It's what we tell our students: you don't have (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations