Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Biases in use of positive and negative words across twenty natural languages.Paul Rozin, Loren Berman & Edward Royzman - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (3):536-548.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Age-group differences in interference from young and older emotional faces.Natalie C. Ebner & Marcia K. Johnson - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (7):1095-1116.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Emotion and culture: A meta-analysis.Dianne A. van Hemert, Ype H. Poortinga & Fons J. R. van de Vijver - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (5):913-943.
    A meta-analysis of 190 cross-cultural emotion studies, published between 1967 and 2000, was performed to examine (1) to what extent reported cross-cultural differences in emotion variables could be regarded as valid (substantive factors) or as method-related (statistical artefacts, cultural bias), and (2) which country characteristics could explain valid cross-cultural differences in emotion. The relative contribution of substantive and method-related factors at sample, study, and country level was investigated and country-level explanations for differences in emotions were tested. Results indicate that a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Age and emotion affect how we look at a face: Visual scan patterns differ for own-age versus other-age emotional faces.Natalie C. Ebner, Yi He & Marcia K. Johnson - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (6):983-997.
    We investigated how age of faces and emotion expressed in faces affect young (n=30) and older (n=20) adults’ visual inspection while viewing faces and judging their expressions. Overall, expression identification was better for young than older faces, suggesting that interpreting expressions in young faces is easier than in older faces, even for older participants. Moreover, there were age-group differences in misattributions of expressions, in that young participants were more likely to label disgusted faces as angry, whereas older adults were more (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Visual search of emotional faces: The role of affective content and featural distinctiveness.Manuel G. Calvo & Hipólito Marrero - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (4):782-806.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review.Sander L. Koole - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (1):4-41.
    The present article reviews modern research on the psychology of emotion regulation. Emotion regulation determines the offset of emotional responding and is thus distinct from emotional sensitivity, which determines the onset of emotional responding. Among the most viable categories for classifying emotion-regulation strategies are the targets and functions of emotion regulation. The emotion-generating systems that are targeted in emotion regulation include attention, knowledge, and bodily responses. The functions of emotion regulation include satisfying hedonic needs, supporting specific goal pursuits, and facilitating (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   101 citations  
  • Age differences in affective forecasting and experienced emotion surrounding the 2008 US presidential election.Susanne Scheibe, Rui Mata & Laura L. Carstensen - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (6):1029-1044.
    In everyday life, people frequently make decisions based on tacit or explicit forecasts about the emotional consequences associated with the possible choices. We investigated age differences in such forecasts and their accuracy by surveying voters about their expected and, subsequently, their actual emotional responses to the 2008 US presidential election. A sample of 762 Democratic and Republican voters aged 20 to 80 years participated in a web-based study; 346 could be re-contacted two days after the election. Older adults forecasted lower (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Deliberate real-time mood regulation in adulthood: The importance of age, fixation and attentional functioning.Soo Rim Noh, Monika Lohani & Derek M. Isaacowitz - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (6):998-1013.
    While previous research has linked executive attention to emotion regulation, the current study investigated the role of attentional alerting (i.e., efficient use of external warning cues) on younger (N=39) and older (N=44) adults’ use of gaze to regulate their mood in real time. Participants viewed highly arousing unpleasant images while reporting their mood and were instructed to deliberately manage how they felt and to minimise the effect of those stimuli on their mood. Fixations toward the most negative areas of the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Culture, gender, and the bipolarity of momentary affect: A critical re-examination.Ulrich Schimmack - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (3):599-604.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Emotional-stimulus processing in trait anxiety is modulated by stimulus valence during neuroimaging of a working-memory task.Christina L. Fales, Karla E. Becerril, Katherine R. Luking & Deanna M. Barch - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (2):200-222.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Global-happy and local-sad: Perceptual processing affects emotion identification.Narayanan Srinivasan & Asma Hanif - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (6):1062-1069.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Categorical perception of anger is disrupted in alexithymia: Evidence from a visual ERP study.Nicolas Vermeulen, Olivier Luminet, Mariana Cordovil de Sousa & Salvatore Campanella - 2008 - Cognition and Emotion 22 (6):1052-1067.
    High and low alexithymia scorers were confronted with a modified visual oddball task that allowed the study of categorical perception of emotional expressions on faces. Participants had to quickly detect a deviant (rare) morphed face that shared or did not share the same emotional expression as the frequent one. Expected categorical perception effects, which were also neurophysiologically indexed, showed that rare stimuli were detected faster if they depicted a different emotional expression compared to rare stimuli depicting the same emotional expression (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • I love you but … : Cultural differences in complexity of emotional experience during interaction with a romantic partner.Michelle N. Shiota, Belinda Campos, Gian C. Gonzaga, Dacher Keltner & Kaiping Peng - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (5):786-799.
    Studies suggest that emotional complexity—the experience of positive and negative emotion in response to the same event—is unusual in Western samples. However, recent research finds that the co-occurrence of positive and negative emotion during unstructured situations is more common among East Asians than Westerners, consistent with theories emphasising the prevalence of dialectical folk epistemology in East-Asian culture. The present study builds upon previous research by examining Asian- and European-Americans' experience of a particular positive emotion—love—and a situationally appropriate negative emotion during (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Age effects and gaze patterns in recognising emotional expressions: An in-depth look at gaze measures and covariates.Nora A. Murphy & Derek M. Isaacowitz - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (3):436-452.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Interpretive bias in social phobia: An ERP study with morphed emotional schematic faces.Iris-Tatjana Kolassa, Stephan Kolassa, Sandra Bergmann, Romy Lauche, Stefan Dilger, Wolfgang Hr Miltner & Frauke Musial - 2009 - Cognition and Emotion 23 (1):69-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Weakened Links Between Mind and Body in Older Age: The Case for Maturational Dualism in the Experience of Emotion.Wendy Berry Mendes - 2010 - Emotion Review 2 (3):240-244.
    As neuroscience methods begin to dominate emotion research it is critical for researchers to remember that peripheral embodiments are critical to understanding emotional experience and emotion—behavior links. Much of modern emotion research assumes reliable mind—body connections that suggest that changes in emotional states influence bodily responses and, vice versa, that somatovisceral information shapes emotional experiences. However, there may be important qualifications to the link between the mind and the (peripheral) body. For example, the ability to sense internal and external bodily (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Beyond “happy, angry, or sad?”: Age-of-poser and age-of-rater effects on multi-dimensional emotion perception.Michaela Riediger, Manuel C. Voelkle, Natalie C. Ebner & Ulman Lindenberger - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (6):968-982.
    Young, middle-aged, and older raters (N=154) evaluated 1,026 prototypical facial poses of neutrality, happiness, anger, disgust, fear, and sadness stemming from 171 young, middle-aged, and older posers. The majority of poses were rated as multi-faceted, that is, to comprise several expressions of varying intensities. Consistent with the notion of age-related increases in negativity–avoidance/positivity effects, crossed-random effects analyses showed an age-related decrease in the attributions of negative, but not positive and neutral, target expressions (that the poser intended to show), and an (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Negative emotional outcomes impair older adults’ reversal learning.Kaoru Nashiro, Mara Mather, Marissa A. Gorlick & Lin Nga - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (6):1014-1028.
    In a typical reversal-learning experiment, one learns stimulus–outcome contingencies that then switch without warning. For instance, participants might have to repeatedly choose between two faces, one of which yields points whereas the other does not, with a reversal at some point in which face yields points. The current study examined age differences in the effects of outcome type on reversal learning. In the first experiment, the participants’ task was either to select the person who would be in a better mood (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Measuring reversal learning: Introducing the Variable Iowa Gambling Task in a study of young and old normals.Stephanie Kovalchik & John Allman - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (5):714-728.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Sex differences in scanning faces: Does attention to the eyes explain female superiority in facial expression recognition?Jessica K. Hall, Sam B. Hutton & Michael J. Morgan - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (4):629-637.
    Previous meta-analyses support a female advantage in decoding non-verbal emotion (Hall, 1978, 1984), yet the mechanisms underlying this advantage are not understood. The present study examined whether the female advantage is related to greater female attention to the eyes. Eye-tracking techniques were used to measure attention to the eyes in 19 males and 20 females during a facial expression recognition task. Women were faster and more accurate in their expression recognition compared with men, and women looked more at the eyes (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations