Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Limits on Monolingualism? A Comparison of Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Abilities to Integrate Lexical Tone in Novel Word Learning.Leher Singh, Felicia L. S. Poh & Charlene S. L. Fu - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7:188260.
    To construct their first lexicon, infants must determine the relationship between native phonological variation and the meanings of words. This process is arguably more complex for bilingual learners who are often confronted with phonological conflict: phonological variation that is lexically relevant in one language may be lexically irrelevant in the other. In a series of four experiments, the present study investigated English–Mandarin bilingual infants’ abilities to negotiate phonological conflict introduced by learning both a tone and a non-tone language. In a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Is it or isn’t it: Listeners make rapid use of prosody to infer speaker meanings.Chigusa Kurumada, Meredith Brown, Sarah Bibyk, Daniel F. Pontillo & Michael K. Tanenhaus - 2014 - Cognition 133 (2):335-342.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Influences of lexical tone and pitch on word recognition in bilingual infants.Leher Singh & Joanne Foong - 2012 - Cognition 124 (2):128-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Constraints on Tone Sensitivity in Novel Word Learning by Monolingual and Bilingual Infants: Tone Properties Are More Influential than Tone Familiarity.Denis Burnham, Leher Singh, Karen Mattock, Pei J. Woo & Marina Kalashnikova - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • How Native Prosody Affects Pitch Processing during Word Learning in Limburgian and Dutch Toddlers and Adults.Stefanie Ramachers, Susanne Brouwer & Paula Fikkert - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8:290015.
    In this study, Limburgian and Dutch 2,5- to 4-year-olds and adults took part in a word learning experiment. Following the procedure employed by Quam and Swingley (2010) and Singh et al. (2014), participants learned two novel word-object mappings. After training, word recognition was tested in correct pronunciation (CP) trials and mispronunciation (MP) trials featuring a pitch change. Since Limburgian is considered a restricted tone language, we expected that the pitch change would hinder word recognition in Limburgian, but not in non-tonal (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Spoken word recognition in early childhood: Comparative effects of vowel, consonant and lexical tone variation.Leher Singh, Hwee Hwee Goh & Thilanga D. Wewalaarachchi - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):1-11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • The developmental course of lexical tone perception in the first year of life.Karen Mattock, Monika Molnar, Linda Polka & Denis Burnham - 2008 - Cognition 106 (3):1367-1381.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children.Daniel Swingley & Richard N. Aslin - 2000 - Cognition 76 (2):147-166.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations