Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. (1 other version)The Effect of Information Overlap on Communication Effectiveness.Shali Wu & Boaz Keysar - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (1):169-181.
    It makes sense that the more information people share, the better they communicate. To evaluate the effect of knowledge overlap on the effectiveness of communication, participants played a communication game where the “director” identified objects to the “addressee”. Pairs either shared information about most objects' names (high overlap), or about the minority of objects' names (low overlap). We found that high-overlap directors tended to use more names than low overlap directors. High overlap directors also used more names with objects whose (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Does bodily awareness interfere with highly skilled movement?Barbara Montero - 2010 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 53 (2):105 – 122.
    It is widely thought that focusing on highly skilled movements while performing them hinders their execution. Once you have developed the ability to tee off in golf, play an arpeggio on the piano, or perform a pirouette in ballet, attention to what your body is doing is thought to lead to inaccuracies, blunders, and sometimes even utter paralysis. Here I re-examine this view and argue that it lacks support when taken as a general thesis. Although bodily awareness may often interfere (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   68 citations  
  • We-Intentions.Raimo Tuomela & Kaarlo Miller - 1988 - Philosophical Studies 53 (3):367-389.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   170 citations  
  • Being-in-the-flow: expert coping as beyond both thought and automaticity.Joshua A. Bergamin - 2017 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 16 (3):403-424.
    Hubert Dreyfus argues that explicit thought disrupts smooth coping at both the level of everyday tasks and of highly-refined skills. However, Barbara Montero criticises Dreyfus for extending what she calls the ‘principle of automaticity’ from our everyday actions to those of trained experts. In this paper, I defend Dreyfus’ account while refining his phenomenology. I examine the phenomenology of what I call ‘esoteric’ expertise to argue that the explicit thought Montero invokes belongs rather to ‘gaps’ between or above moments of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Knowing Other People: A Second‐Person Framework.Bonnie M. Talbert - 2014 - Ratio 28 (2):190-206.
    What does it mean to know another person, and how is such knowledge different from other kinds of knowledge? These questions constitute an important part of what I call ‘second-person epistemology’ – the study of how we know other people. I claim that knowledge of other people is not only central to our everyday lives, but it is a kind of knowledge that is unlike other kinds of knowledge. In general, I will argue that second-person knowledge arises from repeated interactions (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Knowing How and Knowing That: The Presidential Address.Gilbert Ryle - 1946 - Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 46:1 - 16.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   216 citations  
  • Regarding the pain of others.Susan Sontag - 2003 - Diogène 201 (1):127-.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   126 citations  
  • (1 other version)Being and Nothingness.Frederick A. Olafson, Jean-Paul Sartre & Hazel E. Barnes - 1958 - Philosophical Review 67 (2):276.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   355 citations  
  • Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention.Shuhong Lin, Boaz Keysar & Nicholas Epley - 2010 - Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (3):551-556.
    People commonly interpret others’ behavior in terms of the actors’ underlying beliefs, knowledge, or other mental states, thereby using their “theory of mind.” Two experiments suggest that using one’s theory of mind is a relatively effortful process. In both experiments, people reflexively used their own knowledge and beliefs to follow a speaker’s instruction, but only effortfully used their theory of mind to take into account a speaker’s intention to interpret those instructions. In Experiment 1, people with lower working memory capacity (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • The Blue and Brown Books.Newton Garver - 1961 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 21 (4):576-577.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   174 citations  
  • Ryle's Knowing how and knowing how to act.Jennifer Hornsby - 2011 - In John Bengson & Marc A. Moffett (eds.), Knowing How: Essays on Knowledge, Mind, and Action. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press USA. pp. 80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • The closeness-communication bias: Increased egocentrism among friends versus strangers.Kenneth Savitsky, Boaz Keysar, Nicholas Epley, Travis Carter & Ashley Swanson - 2011 - Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (1):269-273.
    People commonly believe that they communicate better with close friends than with strangers. We propose, however, that closeness can lead people to overestimate how well they communicate, a phenomenon we term the closeness-communication bias. In one experiment, participants who followed direction of a friend were more likely to make egocentric errors—look at and reach for an object only they could see—than were those who followed direction of a stranger. In two additional experiments, participants who attempted to convey particular meanings with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations