Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Can we analyze Skinner's problem-solving behavior in operant terms?P. C. Dodwell - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):592-593.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The microscopic analysis of behavior: Toward a synthesis of instrumental, perceptual, and cognitive ideas.Stephen Grossberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):594-595.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychology as moral rhetoric.Rom Harré - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):595-596.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • An operant analysis of problem solving.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):583-591.
    Behavior that solves a problem is distinguished by the fact that it changes another part of the solver's behavior and is strengthened when it does so. Problem solving typically involves the construction of discriminative stimuli. Verbal responses produce especially useful stimuli, because they affect other people. As a culture formulates maxims, laws, grammar, and science, its members behave more effectively without direct or prolonged contact with the contingencies thus formulated. The culture solves problems for its members, and does so by (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior.Michael A. Arbib - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):407-436.
    Intermediate constructs are required as bridges between complex behaviors and realistic models of neural circuitry. For cognitive scientists in general, schemas are the appropriate functional units; brain theorists can work with neural layers as units intermediate between structures subserving schemas and small neural circuits.After an account of different levels of analysis, we describe visuomotor coordination in terms of perceptual schemas and motor schemas. The interest of schemas to cognitive science in general is illustrated with the example of perceptual schemas in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • The centrality of instantiations.John A. Barnden - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):437-438.
    This paper is a commentary on the target article by Michael Arbib, “Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior”, in the same issue of the journal, pp. 407–465. -/- I focus on the importance of the inclusion of an ability of a system to entertain, at a given time, multiple instantiations of a given schema (situation template, frame, script, action plan, etc.), and complications introduced into neural/connectionist network systems by such inclusion.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Schemas: Not yet an interlingua for the brain sciences.John K. Tsotsos - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):447-448.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Schema theory: A new approach?W. von Seelen - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):448-449.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Schemas and bridging gaps in the behavioral and brain sciences.Johan P. Wagemans - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):449-450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grasping schemas is (are) difficult.H. T. A. Whiting - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):450-451.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Structure and process in schema-based architectures.Pat Langley - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):442-442.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Eye of toad, and toe of frog?John C. Marshall - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):444-445.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Recent physiological findings on the neuronal circuit of the frog's optic tectum.Nobuyoshi Matsumoto - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):445-446.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Schema theory: A broadening viewpoint.Tang Yi Qun - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (3):446-447.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spanning the levels in cerebellar function.Michael A. Arbib - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):434-435.
    We ask what cerebellum and basal ganglia arguing that cerebellum tunes motor schemas and their coordination. We argue for a synthesis of models addressing the real-time role and error signaling roles of climbing fibers. bridges between regional and neuro-physiological studies, while relates the neurochemis-try of learning to neural and behavioral levels. [CRÉPEL et al.; HOUK et al.; KANO; LINDEN; SIMPSON et al.; SMITH; THACH; VINCENT].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensory prediction as a role for the cerebellum.R. C. Miall, M. Malkmus & E. M. Robertson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):466-467.
    We suggest that the cerebellum generates sensory or estimates based on outgoing motor commands and sensory feedback. Thus, it is not a motor pattern generator (HOUK et al.) but a predictive system which is intimately involved in motor behavior. This theory may explain the sensitivity of the climbing fibers to both unexpected external events and motor errors (SIMPSON et al.), and we speculate that unusual biophysical properties of the inferior olive might allow the cerebellum to develop multiple asynchronous sensory estimates, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • More on climbing fiber signals and their consequence(s).J. I. Simpson, D. R. W. Wylie & C. I. De Zeeuw - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):496-498.
    Several themes can be identified in the commentaries. The first is that the climbing fibers may have more than one function; the second is that the climbing fibers provide sensory rather than motor signals. We accept the possibility that climbing fibers may have more than one function consequence(s)’ in the title. Until we know more about the function of the inhibitory input to the inferior olive from the cerebellar nuclei, which are motor structures, we have to keep open the possibility (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • We know a lot about the cerebellum, but do we know what motor learning is?Stephan P. Swinnen, Charles B. Walter & Natalia Dounskaia - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):474-475.
    In the behavioral literature on human movement, a distinction is made between the learning of parameters and the learning of new movement forms or topologies. Whereas the target articles by Thach, Smith, and Houk et al. provide evidence for cerebellar involvement in parametrization learning and adaptation, the evidence in favor of its involvement in the generation of new movement patterns is less straightforward. A case is made for focusing more attention on the latter issue in the future. This would directly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Eyeblink conditioning, motor control, and the analysis of limbic-cerebellar interactions.Craig Weiss & John F. Disterhoft - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):479-481.
    Several target articles in this BBS special issue address the topic of cerebellar and olivary functions, especially as they pertain to motor earning. Another important topic is the neural interaction between the limbic system and the cerebellum during associative learning. In this commentary we present some of our data on olivo-cerebellar and limbic-cerebellar interactions during eyeblink conditioning. [HOUK et al.; SIMPSON et al.; THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Plasticity of cerebro-cerebellar interactions in patients with cerebellar dysfunction.Karl Wessel - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):481-482.
    Studies comparing movement-related cortical potentials, post-excitatory inhibition after transcranial magnetic brain stimulation, and PET findings in normal controls and patients with cerebellar degeneration demonstrate plasticity of cerebro-cerebellar interactions and hereby support Thach's theory that the cerebellum has the ability to play a role in building behavioral context-response linkages and to build up appropriate responses from simpler constitutive elements, [THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perhaps it's time to completely rethink cerebellar function.James M. Bower - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):438-439.
    The primary assumption made in this series of target articles is that the cerebellum is directly involved in motor control. However, in my opinion, there is ample and growing experimental evidence to question this classical view, whether or not learning is involved. I propose, instead, that the cerebellum is involved in the control of data acquisition for many different sensory systems, [CRÉPEL et al., HOUK et al., SMITH, THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebellar theory out of control.Michael G. Paulin - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):470-471.
    The views of Houk et al., Smith, and Thach on the role of cerebellum in movement control differ substantially, but all three are flawed by the false reasoning that because information passes from the cerebellum to movements the cerebellum must be a movement controller, or a part of one. The divergent and less than compelling ideas expressed by these leading cerebellar theorists epitomize the fruitlessness of this paradigm, and signal the need for a change. [HOUK et al.; SMITH; THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resilient cerebellar theory complies with stiff opposition.Allan M. Smith - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):499-501.
    In response to several requests from commentators, an unambiguous definition of time-varying joint stiffness is provided. However, since a variety of different operations can be used to measure stiffness, a problem for quantification admittedly still exists. Several commentaries pointed out the advantage of controlling joint stiffness in optimizing the speed-accuracy trade-off known as Fittss law. The deficit in rapid reciprocal movements and the impact on joint stiffness inhibition caused by cerebellar lesions is clarified here, as the target article was apparently (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Limitations of PET and lesion studies in defining the role of the human cerebellum in motor learning.D. Timmann & H. C. Diener - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):477-477.
    PET studies using classical conditioning paradigms are reported. It is emphasized that PET studies show and not in learning paradigms. The importance of dissociating motor performance and learning deficits in human lesions studies is demonstrated in two exemplary studies. The different role of the cerebellum in adaptation of postural reflexes and learning of complex voluntary arm movements is discussed, [THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A case study of how a paper containing good ideas, presented by a distinguished scientist, to an appropriate audience, had almost no influence at all.Earl Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):597-598.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Questions raised by the reinforcement paradigm.Anatol Rapoport - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):601-602.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionist Models and Their Properties.J. A. Feldman & D. H. Ballard - 1982 - Cognitive Science 6 (3):205-254.
    Much of the progress in the fields constituting cognitive science has been based upon the use of explicit information processing models, almost exclusively patterned after conventional serial computers. An extension of these ideas to massively parallel, connectionist models appears to offer a number of advantages. After a preliminary discussion, this paper introduces a general connectionist model and considers how it might be used in cognitive science. Among the issues addressed are: stability and noise‐sensitivity, distributed decision‐making, time and sequence problems, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   442 citations  
  • Précis of Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):737-748.
    This bold and brilliant book asks the ultimate question of the life sciences: How did the human mind acquire its incomparable power? In seeking the answer, Merlin Donald traces the evolution of human culture and cognition from primitive apes to the era of artificial intelligence, and presents an original theory of how the human mind evolved from its presymbolic form. In the emergence of modern human culture, Donald proposes, there were three radical transitions. During the first, our bipedal but still (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  • Language and psychological reality: A discussion of Rudolf Botha's study.Peter Slezak - 1981 - Synthese 49 (December):427-439.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):111-32.
    The computational view of mind rests on certain intuitions regarding the fundamental similarity between computation and cognition. We examine some of these intuitions and suggest that they derive from the fact that computers and human organisms are both physical systems whose behavior is correctly described as being governed by rules acting on symbolic representations. Some of the implications of this view are discussed. It is suggested that a fundamental hypothesis of this approach is that there is a natural domain of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   669 citations  
  • The reification of the mind-body problem?Stewart H. Hulse - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):139-140.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Apes have mimetic culture.Robert W. Mitchell & H. Lyn Miles - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):768-768.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Evolution needs a modern theory of the mind.James H. Fetzer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):759-760.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Cultural transitions occur when mind parasites learn new tricks.Liane M. Gabora - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):760-761.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mimetic culture and modern sports: A synthesis.Bruce Bridgeman & Margarita Azmitia - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):751-752.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Archaeology and the cognitive sciences in the study of human evolution.Philip G. Chase - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):752-753.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Negation in Skinner's system.N. E. Wetherick - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):606-607.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cerebellum and cerebral cortex: Contrasting and converging contributions to spatial navigation and memory.Shane M. O'Mara - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):469-470.
    Thach's target article presents a remarkable overview and integration of animal and human studies on the functions of the cerebellum and makes clear theoretical predictions for both the normal operation of the cerebellum and for the effects of cerebellar lesions in the mature human. Commentary is provided on three areas, namely, spatial navigation, implicit learning, and cerebellar agenesis to elicit further development of the themes already present in Thach's paper, [THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How can the cerebellum match “error signal” and “error correction”?Michel Dufossé - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):442-442.
    This study examines how a Purkinje cell receives its appropriate olivary error signal during the learning of compound movements. We suggest that the Purkinje cell only reinforces those target pyramidal cells which already participate in the movement, subsequently reducing any repeated error signal, such as its own climbing fiber input, [simpson et al.; smith].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The notions of joint stiffness and synaptic plasticity in motor memory.Lev P. Latash & Mark L. Latash - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):465-466.
    We criticize the synaptic theory of long-term memory and the inappropriate usage of physical notions such as in motor control theories. Motor control and motor memory hypotheses should be based on explicitly specified hypothetical control variables that are sound from both physiological and physical perspectives. [HOUK et al.; SMITH; THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Voice, gesture and working memory in the emergence of speech.Francisco Aboitiz - 2018 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 19 (1-2):70-85.
    Language and speech depend on a relatively well defined neural circuitry, located predominantly in the left hemisphere. In this article, I discuss the origin of the speech circuit in early humans, as an expansion of an auditory-vocal articulatory network that took place after the last common ancestor with the chimpanzee. I will attempt to converge this perspective with aspects of the Mirror System Hypothesis, particularly those related to the emergence of a meaningful grammar in human communication. Basically, the strengthening of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Computational challenges of evolving the language-ready brain.Michael A. Arbib - 2018 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 19 (1-2):7-21.
    Computational modeling of the macaque brain grounds hypotheses on the brain of LCA-m. Elaborations thereof provide a brain model for LCA-c. The Mirror System Hypothesis charts further steps via imitation and pantomime to protosign and protolanguage on the path to a "language-ready brain" in Homo sapiens, with the path to speech being indirect. The material poses new challenges for both experimentation and modeling.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Semantic Interference and Facilitation: Understanding the Integration of Spatial Distance and Conceptual Similarity During Sentence Reading.Ernesto Guerra & Pia Knoeferle - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How and what does the cerebellum learn?Peter F. C. Gilbert - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):449-450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebellar rhythms: Exploring another metaphor.Patrick D. Roberts, Gin McCollum & Jan E. Holly - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):471-472.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Further evidence for the involvement of nitric oxide in trans-ACPD-induced suppression of AMPA responses in cultured chick Purkinje neurons.Junko Mori-Okamoto & Koichi Okamoto - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):467-468.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cerebellar arm ataxia: Theories still have a lot to explain.J. Hore - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A bridge between cerebellar long-term depression and discrete motor learning: Studies on gene knockout mice.Masanobu Kano - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):488-490.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A cerebellar long-term depression update.David J. Linden - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):482-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Q: Is the cerebellum an adaptive combiner of motor and mental/motor activities? A: Yes, maybe, certainly not, who can say?W. Thomas Thach - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):501-528.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark