- Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.details
|
|
Simulation of expert memory using EPAM IV.Howard B. Richman, James J. Staszewski & Herbert A. Simon - 1995 - Psychological Review 102 (2):305-330.details
|
|
Modules in models of memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-94.details
|
|
Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.details
|
|
How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.details
|
|
Cognitive-Emotional Interactions in the Brain.Joseph E. Ledoux - 1989 - Cognition and Emotion 3 (4):267-289.details
|
|
Does Working-Memory Training Given to Reception-Class Children Improve the Speech of Children at Risk of Fluency Difficulty?Peter Howell, Li Ying Chua, Kaho Yoshikawa, Hannah Hau Shuen Tang, Taniya Welmillage, John Harris & Kevin Tang - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.details
|
|
Memory and mood.Maryanne Martin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-75.details
|
|
Violations of Expectations As Matter for the Believing Process.Hans-Ferdinand Angel & Rüdiger J. Seitz - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.details
|
|
Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.details
|
|
Practice and divided attention.William Hirst - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-73.details
|
|
Modular mind or unitary system: A duck-rabbit effect.Gillian Cohen - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):71-72.details
|
|
Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.details
|
|
Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.details
|
|
The Maltese cross: Simplistic yes, new no.Thomas H. Carr & Tracy L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):69-71.details
|
|
Anxiety and working memory capacity.Shane Darke - 1988 - Cognition and Emotion 2 (2):145-154.details
|
|
What kind of a framework?John Morton - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):75-76.details
|
|
Digit memory of grand experts in abacus-derived mental calculation.Giyoo Hatano & Keiko Osawa - 1983 - Cognition 15 (1-3):95-110.details
|
|
Models of mind: Hidden plumbing.Enoch Callaway - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):68-69.details
|
|
Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.details
|
|
The Maltese cross: A new simplistic model for memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):55-68.details
|
|
Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.details
|
|
The use of interference paradigms as a criterion for separating memory stores.Henry L. Roediger - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):78-79.details
|
|
The usefulness for memory theory of the word “store”.D. J. Murray - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):76-77.details
|
|
The homunculus as bureaucrat.Alan K. Mackworth - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):74-74.details
|
|
Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Wisdom, but not especially unconventional.Robert G. Crowder - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):72-72.details
|
|