Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Are motor images based on kinestheticvisual matching?Robert W. Mitchell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Motor images are action plans.Wolfgang Prinz - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):218-218.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Cognitive and motor implications of mental imagery.Romeo Chua & Daniel J. Weeks - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):203-204.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The storehouse/correspondence partition in memory research: Promises and perils.Arie W. Kruglanski - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):198-199.
    The novel correspondence metaphor outlined by Koriat & Goldsmith offers important advantages for studying critical issues of memory-accuracy. It also fits well with the current emphasis on the reconstructive nature of memory and on the role of cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational factors in memory performance. These positive features notwithstanding, the storehouse/correspondence framework faces potential perils having to do with its implied linkage to the laboratory/real-life controversy and its proposal of studying correspondence issues in isolation from memory phenomena captured by the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Accuracy and quantity are poor measures of recall and recognition.Andrew R. Mayes, Rob van Eijk & Patricia L. Gooding - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):201-202.
    The value of accuracy and quantity as memory measures is assessed. It is argued that (1) accuracy does not measure correspondence (monitoring) because it ignores omissions and correct rejections, (2) quantity is confounded with monitoring in recall, and (3) in recognition, if targets and foils are unequal, both measures, even together, still ignore correct rejections.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory metaphors and the real-life/laboratory controversy: Correspondence versus storehouse conceptions of memory.Asher Koriat & Morris Goldsmith - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):167-188.
    The study of memory is witnessing a spirited clash between proponents of traditional laboratory research and those advocating a more naturalistic approach to the study of “real-life” or “everyday” memory. The debate has generally centered on the “what” (content), “where” (context), and “how” (methods) of memory research. In this target article, we argue that the controversy discloses a further, more fundamental breach between two underlying memory metaphors, each having distinct implications for memory theory and assessment: Whereas traditional memory research has (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • An Empirical and Computational Investigation of Perceiving and Remembering Event Temporal Relations.Shulan Lu, Derek Harter & Arthur C. Graesser - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (3):345-373.
    Events have beginnings, ends, and often overlap in time. A major question is how perceivers come to parse a stream of multimodal information into meaningful units and how different event boundaries may vary event processing. This work investigates the roles of these three types of event boundaries in constructing event temporal relations. Predictions were made based on how people would err according to the beginning state, end state, and overlap heuristic hypotheses. Participants viewed animated events that include all the logical (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Decoupled Representation Theory of the Evolution of Cognition—A Critical Assessment.Wayne Christensen - 2010 - British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 61 (2):361 - 405.
    Sterelny's Thought in a Hostile World ([2003]) presents a complex, systematically structured theory of the evolution of cognition centered on a concept of decoupled representation. Taking Godfrey-Smith's ([1996]) analysis of the evolution of behavioral flexibility as a framework, the theory describes increasingly complex grades of representation beginning with simple detection and culminating with decoupled representation, said to be belief-like, and it characterizes selection forces that drive evolutionary transformations in these forms of representation. Sterelny's ultimate explanatory target is the evolution of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Reporting on Past Psychological States: Beliefs, Desires, and Intentions.Alfred Mele - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):61.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Determining transformation distance in similarity: Considerations for assessing representational changes a priori.Lisa R. Grimm, Jonathan R. Rein & Arthur B. Markman - 2012 - Thinking and Reasoning 18 (1):59 - 80.
    The representational distortion (RD) approach to similarity (e.g., Hahn, Chater, & Richardson, 2003) proposes that similarity is computed using the transformation distance between two entities. We argue that researchers who adopt this approach need to be concerned with how representational transformations can be determined a priori. We discuss several roadblocks to using this approach. Specifically we demonstrate the difficulties inherent in determining what transformations are psychologically salient and the importance of considering the directionality of transformations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The content and acquisition of lexical concepts.Richard Horsey - 2006
    This thesis aims to develop a psychologically plausible account of concepts by integrating key insights from philosophy (on the metaphysical basis for concept possession) and psychology (on the mechanisms underlying concept acquisition). I adopt an approach known as informational atomism, developed by Jerry Fodor. Informational atomism is the conjunction of two theses: (i) informational semantics, according to which conceptual content is constituted exhaustively by nomological mind–world relations; and (ii) conceptual atomism, according to which (lexical) concepts have no internal structure. I (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Solving Bongard Problems With a Visual Language and Pragmatic Constraints.Stefan Depeweg, Contantin A. Rothkopf & Frank Jäkel - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (5):e13432.
    More than 50 years ago, Bongard introduced 100 visual concept learning problems as a challenge for artificial vision systems. These problems are now known as Bongard problems. Although they are well known in cognitive science and artificial intelligence, only very little progress has been made toward building systems that can solve a substantial subset of them. In the system presented here, visual features are extracted through image processing and then translated into a symbolic visual vocabulary. We introduce a formal language (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Scene context automatically drives predictions of object transformations.Giacomo Aldegheri, Surya Gayet & Marius V. Peelen - 2023 - Cognition 238 (C):105521.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarity and structured representation in human and nonhuman apes.Carl J. Hodgetts, James O. E. Close & Ulrike Hahn - 2023 - Cognition 236 (C):105419.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bouba and Kiki inside objects: Sound-shape correspondence for objects with a hole.Sung-Ho Kim - 2020 - Cognition 195 (C):104132.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Universal Visual Features Might Be Necessary for Fluent Reading. A Longitudinal Study of Visual Reading in Braille and Cyrillic Alphabets.Łukasz Bola, Dominika Radziun, Katarzyna Siuda-Krzywicka, Joanna E. Sowa, Małgorzata Paplińska, Ewa Sumera & Marcin Szwed - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From the Lexicon to Expectations About Kinds: A Role for Associative Learning.Eliana Colunga & Linda B. Smith - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (2):347-382.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • The development of contour processing: evidence from physiology and psychophysics.Gemma Taylor, Daniel Hipp, Alecia Moser, Kelly Dickerson & Peter Gerhardstein - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Evolution of Color Vision without Colors.Richard J. Hall - 1996 - Philosophy of Science 63 (S3):S125-S133.
    The standard adaptationist explanation of the presence of a sensory mechanism in an organism—that it detects properties useful to the organism—cannot be given for color vision. This is because colors do not exist. After arguing for this latter claim, I consider, but reject, nonadaptationist explanations. I conclude by proposing an explanation of how color vision could have adaptive value even though it does not detect properties in the environment.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Retinal Images and Object Files: Towards Empirically Evaluating Philosophical Accounts of Visual Perspective.Assaf Weksler - 2016 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 7 (1):91-103.
    According to an influential philosophical view I call “the relational properties view”, “perspectival” properties, such as the elliptical appearance of a tilted coin, are relational properties of external objects. Philosophers have assessed this view on the basis of phenomenological, epistemological or other purely philosophical considerations. My aim in this paper is to examine whether it is possible to evaluate RPV empirically. In the first, negative part of the paper I consider and reject a certain tempting way of doing so. In (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Infants' use of featural and experiential information in segregating and individuating objects: a reply to Xu, Carey and Welch.Amy Needham & Renée Baillargeon - 2000 - Cognition 74 (3):255-284.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Action Alters Shape Categories.Linda B. Smith - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (4):665-679.
    Two experiments show that action alters the shape categories formed by 2-year-olds. Experiment 1 shows that moving an object horizontally (or vertically) defines the horizontal (or vertical) axis as the main axis of elongation and systematically changes the range of shapes seen as similar. Experiment 2 shows that moving an object symmetrically (or asymmetrically) also alters shape categories. Previous work has shown marked developmental changes in object recognition between 1 and 3 years of age. These results suggest a role for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Exemplar similarity and rule application.Ulrike Hahn, Mercè Prat-Sala, Emmanuel M. Pothos & Duncan P. Brumby - 2010 - Cognition 114 (1):1-18.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Categorical perception of familiar objects.Fiona N. Newell & Heinrich H. Bülthoff - 2002 - Cognition 85 (2):113-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Spatial language and spatial representation.William G. Hayward & Michael J. Tarr - 1995 - Cognition 55 (1):39-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Knowing children's minds.Michael Siegal - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):79-80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Unraveling introspection.John Heil - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):49-50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-ascription of belief and desire.Robert M. Gordon - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):45-46.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Towards an ecology of mind.George Butterworth - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):31-32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Plea for more exploration of cross-cultural cognitive space.David Piggins - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):91-92.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Variations in pictorial culture.Arthur C. Danto - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):77-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Real space and represented space: Cross-cultural perspectives.J. B. Deregowski - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):51-74.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Fundamental design limitations in tag assignment.Hermann J. Müller, Glyn W. Humphreys, Philip T. Quinlan & Nick Donnelly - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):410-411.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling separate processing pathways for spatial and object vision.Bruce Bridgeman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (3):398-398.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • From observations on language to theories of visual perception.Johan Wagemans - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):253-254.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Hypothesis testing in experimental and naturalistic memory research.Daniel B. Wright - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):210-211.
    Koriat & Goldsmith's distinction between the correspondence and storehouse metaphors is valuable for both memory theory and methodology. It is questionable, however, whether this distinction underlies the heated debate about so called “everyday memory” research. The distinction between experimental and naturalistic methodologies better characterizes this debate. I compare these distinctions and discuss how the methodological distinction, between experimental and naturalistic designs, could give rise to different theoretical approaches.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory, metamemory, and conditional statistics.Robert A. Bjork & Thomas D. Wickens - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):193-194.
    Koriat & Goldsmith's distinction between encoding processes and metamnemonic decision processes is theoretically and practically important, as is their methodology for separating the two. However, their accuracy measure is a conditional statistic, subject to the unfathomable selection effects that have hindered analogous measures in the past. We also find their arguments concerning basic and applied research mostly beside the point.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Parts and Wholes in Expression Recognition.Murray White - 2000 - Cognition and Emotion 14 (1):39-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Transfer of object category knowledge across visual and haptic modalities: Experimental and computational studies.Ilker Yildirim & Robert A. Jacobs - 2013 - Cognition 126 (2):135-148.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Encoding Shape and Spatial Relations: The Role of Receptive Field Size in Coordinating Complementary Representations.Robert A. Jacobs & Stephen M. Kosslyn - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (3):361-386.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Learning to recognise objects.Guy Wallis & Heinrich Bülthoff - 1999 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 (1):22-31.
    Evidence from neurophysiological and psychological studies is coming together to shed light on how we represent and recognize objects. This review describes evidence supporting two major hypotheses: the first is that objects are represented in a mosaic-like form in which objects are encoded by combinations of complex, reusable features, rather than two-dimensional templates, or three-dimensional models. The second hypothesis is that transform-invariant representations of objects are learnt through experience, and that this learning is affected by the temporal sequence in which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The what and why of binding: The modeler's perspective.Christoph von der Malsburg - 1999 - Neuron 24:95-104.
    In attempts to formulate a computational understanding of brain function, one of the fundamental concerns is the data structure by which the brain represents information. For many decades, a conceptual framework has dominated the thinking of both brain modelers and neurobiologists. That framework is referred to here as "classical neural networks." It is well supported by experimental data, although it may be incomplete. A characterization of this framework will be offered in the next section. Difficulties in modeling important functional aspects (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Evolutionary Computation for Modelling Social Traits in Realistic Looking Synthetic Faces.Felix Fuentes-Hurtado, Jose A. Diego-Mas, Valery Naranjo & Mariano Alcañiz - 2018 - Complexity 2018:1-16.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Object completion effects in attention and memory.Siyi Chen - 2018 - Dissertation, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Bayesian hierarchical grouping: Perceptual grouping as mixture estimation.Vicky Froyen, Jacob Feldman & Manish Singh - 2015 - Psychological Review 122 (4):575-597.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Frames of reference in unilateral neglect and visual perception: A computational perspective.Michael C. Mozer - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (1):156-185.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Do semantic contextual cues facilitate transfer learning from video in toddlers?Laura Zimmermann, Alecia Moser, Amanda Grenell, Kelly Dickerson, Qianwen Yao, Peter Gerhardstein & Rachel Barr - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:130634.
    Young children typically demonstrate a transfer deficit, learning less from video than live presentations. Semantically meaningful context has been demonstrated to enhance learning in young children. We examined the effect of a semantically meaningful context on toddlers’ imitation performance. Two- and 2.5-year-olds participated in a puzzle imitation task to examine learning from either a live or televised model. The model demonstrated how to assemble a three-piece puzzle to make a fish or a boat, with the puzzle demonstration occurring against a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The brain’s cutting-room floor: segmentation of narrative cinema.Jeffrey M. Zacks, Nicole K. Speer, Khena M. Swallow & Corey John Maley - unknown
    This Document is Protected by copyright and was first published by Frontiers. All rights reserved. it is reproduced with permission.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations