Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Advancing the rationality debate.Keith E. Stanovich & Richard F. West - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5):701-717.
    In this response, we clarify several misunderstandings of the understanding/acceptance principle and defend our specific operationalization of that principle. We reiterate the importance of addressing the problem of rational task construal and we elaborate the notion of computational limitations contained in our target article. Our concept of thinking dispositions as variable intentional-level styles of epistemic and behavioral regulation is explained, as is its relation to the rationality debate. Many of the suggestions of the commentators for elaborating two-process models are easily (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Offloading information to an external store increases false recall.Xinyi Lu, Megan O. Kelly & Evan F. Risko - 2020 - Cognition 205 (C):104428.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Gaze patterns reveal how situation models and text representations contribute to episodic text memory.Roger Johansson, Franziska Oren & Kenneth Holmqvist - 2018 - Cognition 175 (C):53-68.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sum-Difference Theory of Remembering and Knowing: A Two-Dimensional Signal-Detection Model.Caren M. Rotello, Neil A. Macmillan & John A. Reeder - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (3):588-616.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Can quantum probability provide a new direction for cognitive modeling?Emmanuel M. Pothos & Jerome R. Busemeyer - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (3):255-274.
    Classical (Bayesian) probability (CP) theory has led to an influential research tradition for modeling cognitive processes. Cognitive scientists have been trained to work with CP principles for so long that it is hard even to imagine alternative ways to formalize probabilities. However, in physics, quantum probability (QP) theory has been the dominant probabilistic approach for nearly 100 years. Could QP theory provide us with any advantages in cognitive modeling as well? Note first that both CP and QP theory share the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • Superposition of Episodic Memories: Overdistribution and Quantum Models.Charles J. Brainerd, Zheng Wang & Valerie F. Reyna - 2013 - Topics in Cognitive Science 5 (4):773-799.
    Memory exhibits episodic superposition, an analog of the quantum superposition of physical states: Before a cue for a presented or unpresented item is administered on a memory test, the item has the simultaneous potential to occupy all members of a mutually exclusive set of episodic states, though it occupies only one of those states after the cue is administered. This phenomenon can be modeled with a nonadditive probability model called overdistribution (OD), which implements fuzzy-trace theory's distinction between verbatim and gist (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • A matter of focus: Detailed memory in the intentional autobiographical recall of older and younger adults.Alaitz Aizpurua & Wilma Koutstaal - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33:145-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What's in a name for memory errors? Implications and ethical issues arising from the use of the term "false memory" for errors in memory for details.Anne P. DePrince, Carolyn B. Allard, Hannah Oh & Jennifer J. Freyd - 2004 - Ethics and Behavior 14 (3):201 – 233.
    The term "false memories" has been used to refer to suggestibility experiments in which whole events are apparently confabulated and in media accounts of contested memories of childhood abuse. Since 1992 psychologists have increasingly used the term "false memory" when discussing memory errors for details, such as specific words within word lists. Use of the term to refer to errors in details is a shift in language away from other terms used historically (e.g., "memory intrusions"). We empirically examine this shift (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Emotional valence of stimuli modulates false recognition: Using a modified version of the simplified conjoint recognition paradigm.Xianmin Gong, Hongrui Xiao & Dahua Wang - 2016 - Cognition 156 (C):95-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A context noise model of episodic word recognition.Simon Dennis & Michael S. Humphreys - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (2):452-478.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Recollection rejection: False-memory editing in children and adults.C. J. Brainerd, V. F. Reyna, Ron Wright & A. H. Mojardin - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (4):762-784.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Recollective and non-recollective processes in working memory retrieval.Fiona Laura Rosselet-Jordan, Marlène Abadie, Stéphanie Mariz Elsig, Pierre Barrouillet & Valérie Camos - 2025 - Cognition 254 (C):105978.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Measuring recollection and familiarity: Improving the remember/know procedure.Ellen M. Migo, Andrew R. Mayes & Daniela Montaldi - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (3):1435-1455.
    The remember/know procedure is the most widely used method to investigate recollection and familiarity. It uses trial-by-trial reports to determine how much recollection and familiarity contribute to different kinds of recognition. Few other methods provide information about individual memory judgements and no alternative allows such direct indications of recollection and familiarity influences. Here we review how the RK procedure has been and should be used to help resolve theoretical disagreements about the processing and neural bases of components of recognition memory. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The differential similarity of positive and negative information – an affect-induced processing outcome?Hans Alves, Alex Koch & Christian Unkelbach - 2019 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (6):1224-1238.
    People judge positive information to be more alike than negative information. This good-bad asymmetry in similarity was argued to constitute a true property of the information ecology (Alves, H., Koch, A., & Unkelbach, C. (2017). Why good is more alike than bad: Processing implications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 69–79). Alternatively, the asymmetry may constitute a processing outcome itself, namely an influence of phasic affect on information processing. Because no research has yet tested whether phasic affect influences perceived similarity among (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Electrophysiological evidence for the effects of emotional content on false recognition memory.Zhiwei Zheng, Minjia Lang, Wei Wang, Fengqiu Xiao & Juan Li - 2018 - Cognition 179 (C):298-310.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A dynamic approach to recognition memory.Gregory E. Cox & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2017 - Psychological Review 124 (6):795-860.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Generalized Quantum Episodic Memory Model.Jennifer S. Trueblood & Pernille Hemmer - 2017 - Cognitive Science:2089-2125.
    Recent evidence suggests that experienced events are often mapped to too many episodic states, including those that are logically or experimentally incompatible with one another. For example, episodic over-distribution patterns show that the probability of accepting an item under different mutually exclusive conditions violates the disjunction rule. A related example, called subadditivity, occurs when the probability of accepting an item under mutually exclusive and exhaustive instruction conditions sums to a number >1. Both the over-distribution effect and subadditivity have been widely (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biased confabulation in risky choice.Alice Mason, Christopher R. Madan, Nick Simonsen, Marcia L. Spetch & Elliot A. Ludvig - 2022 - Cognition 229 (C):105245.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can people strategically control the encoding and retrieval of some morphologic and typographic details of words?Jerwen Jou & Hector M. Cortes - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (3):1280-1297.
    This study investigated whether the encoding and retrieval of plurality information and letter-case information of words in recognition memory can be inhibited. Response-deadline experiments using single words have indicted a controlled processing mode, whereas studies using meaningful sentences have indicated an automatic mode of processing plurality information. Two similar opposing views have existed on the processing of letter-case information. The abstractionist view contends that we retain the abstract lexical information and discard the superficial perceptual case information. The proceduralist view holds (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Trajectory of Targets and Critical Lures in the Deese/Roediger–McDermott Paradigm: A Systematic Review.Patricia I. Coburn, Kirandeep K. Dogra, Iarenjit K. Rai & Daniel M. Bernstein - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    The Deese/Roediger–McDermott paradigm has been used extensively to examine false memory. During the study session, participants learn lists of semantically related items, referred to as targets. Critical lures are items which are also associated with the lists but are intentionally omitted from study. At test, when asked to remember targets, participants often report false memories for critical lures. Findings from experiments using the DRM show the ease with which false memories develop in the absence of suggestion or misinformation. Given this, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Manifestations and Consequences of Negative Information’s Great Diversity.Hans Alves - unknown
    In the present dissertation, I propose a general, robust, and objective characteristic of the information environment, according to which negative information is more diverse than positive information. I present an explanatory framework for this phenomenon based on the non-extremity of positive qualities. Specifically, most attribute dimensions host one “positive” range which is surrounded by two distinct “negative” ranges, resulting in a greater diversity of negative compared to positive attributes, stimuli, and information in general. Chapter 1 of my dissertation reviews evidence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Models of memory.Jeroen Gw Raaijmakers & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2002 - In J. Wixted & H. Pashler (eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Rational analysis as a link between human memory and information retrieval.Mark Steyvers & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2008 - In Nick Chater & Mike Oaksford (eds.), The Probabilistic Mind: Prospects for Bayesian Cognitive Science. Oxford University Press. pp. 329--349.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations