- A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data selection.Mike Oaksford & Nick Chater - 1994 - Psychological Review 101 (4):608-631.details
|
|
Rational analysis, intractability, and the prospects of ‘as if’-explanations.Iris van Rooij, Johan Kwisthout, Todd Wareham & Cory Wright - 2018 - Synthese 195 (2):491-510.details
|
|
How connectionist models learn: The course of learning in connectionist networks.John K. Kruschke - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):498-499.details
|
|
Learning from learned networks.M. Pavel - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):503-504.details
|
|
Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: A critique of the “locality” assumption.Martha J. Farah - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):43-61.details
|
|
The Role of Falsification in the Development of Cognitive Architectures: Insights from a Lakatosian Analysis.Richard P. Cooper - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (3):509-533.details
|
|
(1 other version)Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):417-437.details
|
|
Against Logicist Cognitive Science.Mike Oaksford & Nick Chater - 1991 - Mind and Language 6 (1):1-38.details
|
|
Classical and connectionist models: Levels of description.Josep E. Corbí - 1993 - Synthese 95 (2):141 - 168.details
|
|
The rational analysis of mind and behavior.Nick Chater & Mike Oaksford - 2000 - Synthese 122 (1-2):93-131.details
|
|
Connectionism, classical cognitive science and experimental psychology.Mike Oaksford, Nick Chater & Keith Stenning - 1990 - AI and Society 4 (1):73-90.details
|
|
Philosophy and Memory Traces: Descartes to Connectionism.John Sutton - 1998 - New York: Cambridge University Press.details
|
|
Probabilistic single function dual process theory and logic programming as approaches to non-monotonicity in human vs. artificial reasoning.Mike Oaksford & Nick Chater - 2014 - Thinking and Reasoning 20 (2):269-295.details
|
|
Go with the flow but mind the details.Glyn W. Humphreys & M. Jane Riddoch - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):71-72.details
|
|
Perception and its interactive substrate: Psychophysical linking hypotheses and psychophysical methods.Robert Sekuler - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):79-79.details
|
|
Toward a Connectionist Model of Recursion in Human Linguistic Performance.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (2):157-205.details
|
|
The imaginary fundamentalists: The unshocking truth about Bayesian cognitive science.Nick Chater, Noah Goodman, Thomas L. Griffiths, Charles Kemp, Mike Oaksford & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (4):194-196.details
|
|
What connectionist models learn: Learning and representation in connectionist networks.Stephen José Hanson & David J. Burr - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):471-489.details
|
|
Throwing out the neuropsychological data with the locality bathwater?Philip Servos & Elizabeth M. Olds - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):80-81.details
|
|
Casting one's net too widely?D. P. Carey & A. D. Milner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):65-66.details
|
|
Is the Cerebral Neocortex a Uniform Cognitive Architecture?Martin Ebdon - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (3):368-395.details
|
|
(1 other version)Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.M. H. Christiansen, N. Chater & M. S. Seidenberg - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):417-437.details
|
|
A non-empiricist perspective on learning in layered networks.Michael I. Jordan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):497-498.details
|
|
There is more to learning then meeth the eye.Noel E. Sharkey - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):506-507.details
|
|
Two projects for understanding the mind: A response to Morris and Richardson. [REVIEW]Nick Chater & Martin Pickering - 1997 - Minds and Machines 7 (4):553-569.details
|
|
Advances in neural network theory.Gérard Toulouse - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-509.details
|
|
Problems of extension, representation, and computational irreducibility.Patrick Suppes - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):507-508.details
|
|
The functional architecture of visual attention may still be modular.Carlo Umiltà - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):82-83.details
|
|
The Computational Origin of Representation.Steven T. Piantadosi - 2020 - Minds and Machines 31 (1):1-58.details
|
|
Connectionist models learn what?Timothy van Gelder - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):509-510.details
|
|
Representational systems and symbolic systems.Gordon D. A. Brown & Mike Oaksford - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):492-493.details
|
|
No threat to modularity.Yosef Grodzinsky & Uri Hadar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):70-71.details
|
|
Towards a connectionist cognitive architecture.Keith Butler - 1991 - Mind and Language 6 (3):252-72.details
|
|
On learnability, empirical foundations, and naturalness.W. J. M. Levelt - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):501-501.details
|
|
The Allure of Connectionism Reexamined.B. P. McLaughlin & T. A. Warfield - 1994 - Synthese 101 (3):365 - 400.details
|
|
Précis of simple heuristics that make us Smart.Peter M. Todd & Gerd Gigerenzer - 2000 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5):727-741.details
|
|
Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.details
|
|
The localization/distribution distinction in neuropsychology is related to the isomorphism/multiple meaning distinction in cell electrophysiology.Gerald S. Wasserman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):87-88.details
|
|
Local and distributed processes in attentional orienting.Michael I. Posner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):78-79.details
|
|
Keeping representations at bay.Stanley Munsat - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):502-503.details
|
|
The allure of connectionism reexamined.Brian P. McLaughlin & F. Warfield - 1994 - Synthese 101 (3):365-400.details
|
|
Do neuropsychologists think in terms of interactive models?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):72-73.details
|
|
Theories of reasoning and the computational explanation of everyday inference.Mike Oaksford & Nick Chater - 1995 - Thinking and Reasoning 1 (2):121 – 152.details
|
|
Learning and representation: Tensions at the interface.Steven José Hanson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):511-518.details
|
|
Discarding locality assumptions: Problems and prospects.Ruth Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):64-65.details
|
|
Is logicist cognitive science possible?Alan Garnham - 1993 - Mind and Language 8 (1):49-71.details
|
|
Neural constraints in cognitive science.Keith Butler - 1994 - Minds and Machines 4 (2):129-62.details
|
|
Connectionism, classical cognitivism and the relation between cognitive and implementational levels of analysis.Keith Butler - 1993 - Philosophical Psychology 6 (3):321-33.details
|
|
But what is the substance of connectionist representation?James Hendler - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):496-497.details
|
|
Realistic neural nets need to learn iconic representations.W. A. Phillips, P. J. B. Hancock & L. S. Smith - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):505-505.details
|
|