Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Backchannels in the lab and in the wild.Allison Nguyen, Andrew J. Guydish & Jean E. Fox Tree - 2024 - Interaction Studies 25 (1):70-99.
    Backchannel choices affect conversational development. Some backchannels invite interlocutors to continue to the next part of what they are saying and others invite them to elaborate on what they have just said. We tested how communicative modality (audiovisual, audio, text), environmental setting (wholly in-lab, partially in the wild), and conversational goals (on-task, off-task) influenced backchannel usage by participants. We found that backchannel production depends on modality, setting, and goals. For example, we found that specific backchannels played a more prominent role (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Overhearers Use Addressee Backchannels in Dialog Comprehension.Jackson Tolins & Jean E. Fox Tree - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (6):1412-1434.
    Observing others in conversation is a common format for comprehending language, yet little work has been done to understand dialog comprehension. We tested whether overhearers use addressee backchannels as predictive cues for how to integrate information across speaker turns during comprehension of spontaneously produced collaborative narration. In Experiment 1, words that followed specific backchannels were recognized more slowly than words that followed either generic backchannels or pauses. In Experiment 2, we found that when the turn after the backchannel was a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Interactional strategies for progressing through quizzes in dementia settings.Val Williams, Camilla Lindholm & Joseph Webb - 2020 - Discourse Studies 22 (4):503-522.
    People with early-to-mid stage dementia frequently attend groups that provide opportunities for socialising and engaging in group activities, such as quizzes. This article uses conversation analysis to investigate the interactional strategies that the staff use to initiate and keep these quizzes ‘on track’, and what they orient to as impediments and facilitators of quiz progression. Specifically, we outline how staff deal with incorrect or ‘non-answers’, and what happens when players have their own goals or ‘projects’ that do not align with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Integrating conversation analysis and issue framing to illuminate collaborative decision-making activities.Christina Wasson - 2016 - Discourse and Communication 10 (4):378-411.
    A shift from top-down, hierarchical decision-making toward collaborative, consensus-oriented decision-making is taking place across many settings, leading to meetings in which diverse participants seek to reach agreement on issues of significance. This article proposes a new approach to analyzing such meetings that integrates conversation analysis and issue framing. While CA and IF have both been applied to collaborative decision-making, each approach, on its own, suffers from significant limitations. Combined, they allow negotiation talk in meetings to be examined holistically, integrating a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Book Reviews. [REVIEW]Laurel Smith Stvan, Fiona Glade, Deborah Hurlock & Johanna Rendle-Short - 2004 - Discourse Studies 6 (2):277-284.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Influence of Robot Verbal Support on Human Team Members: Encouraging Outgroup Contributions and Suppressing Ingroup Supportive Behavior.Sarah Sebo, Ling Liang Dong, Nicholas Chang, Michal Lewkowicz, Michael Schutzman & Brian Scassellati - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    As teams of people increasingly incorporate robot members, it is essential to consider how a robot's actions may influence the team's social dynamics and interactions. In this work, we investigated the effects of verbal support from a robot on human team members' interactions related to psychological safety and inclusion. We conducted a between-subjects experiment where the robot team member either gave verbal support or did not give verbal support to the human team members of a human-robot team comprised of 2 (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How is emotional resonance achieved in storytellings of sadness/distress?Christoph Rühlemann - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:952119.
    Storytelling pivots around stance seen as a window unto emotion: storytellers project a stance expressing their emotion toward the events and recipients preferably mirror that stance by affiliating with the storyteller’s stance. Whether the recipient’s affiliative stance is at the same time expressive of his/her emotional resonance with the storyteller and of emotional contagion is a question that has recently attracted intriguing research in Physiological Interaction Research. Connecting to this line of inquiry, this paper concerns itself with storytellings of sadness/distress. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Doing empathy and sympathy: caring responses to troubles tellings on a peer support line.Christopher Pudlinski - 2005 - Discourse Studies 7 (3):267-288.
    Conversation analysis of 53 emotive responses to troubles tellings on a peer support line discovered eight different methods for expressing empathy and/or sympathy. Emotive reactions, assessments, and formulating the gist of the trouble typically occur early on in a troubles telling. Reporting one’s own reaction was found in the midst of troubles telling, as a second reaction to ‘bad’ news or after callers’ reports of their own feelings. Naming another’s feelings and using an idiom occur towards the end of a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Encouraging responses to good news on a peer support line.Christopher Pudlinski - 2008 - Discourse Studies 10 (6):795-812.
    When callers to a warm line report on good news tied to a current or ongoing problem, call takers use four different methods to endorse this good news and encourage actions implied within the report. Building upon 93 potentially encouraging responses in 65 actual warm line calls, this study describes four different methods of encouragement used by call takers within news delivery sequences: 1) positive assessments; 2) assessments plus formulations; 3) statements of agreement to a planned action; and 4) second (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Common Understandings of and Consensus About Collective Action: The Transformation of Specifically Vague Proposals as a Collective Achievement.Ole Pütz - 2019 - Human Studies 42 (3):483-512.
    This paper asks how anti-nuclear activists form collectives that are able to act collectively. It argues that shared interests and collective identities only insufficiently explain the emergence of collective action. Alternatively, the paper investigates meeting talk of German anti-nuclear groups where activists discuss proposals for collective action. Based on audio recordings, a sequential analysis of activists’ deliberations traces the transformation of vague ideas into concrete and collectively agreed to proposals. It is shown how the process by which activists reach a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Blame attributions and mitigated confessions: The discursive construction of guilty admissions in celebrity TV confessionals.Ruth Parry & Wendy Archer - 2019 - Discourse and Communication 13 (6):591-611.
    Drawing on insights from conversation analysis, discursive psychology and social psychology, this article describes some interactional features of two celebrity TV confessionals and the resources used by the TV interviewers and celebrity guests to attribute, accept or deny responsibility for their transgressions. The analytic interest lies in how confessions are locally and interactionally managed, that is, how ‘doing confessing’ is achieved in the television interview context. We show how the host’s opening turn constrains the celebrity guest’s contribution and secures overt (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Particle Jako (“Like”) in Spoken Czech: From Expressing Comparison to Mobilizing Affiliative Responses.Florence Oloff - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    This contribution investigates the use of the Czech particle jako in naturally occurring conversations. Inspired by interactional research on unfinished or suspended utterances and on turn-final conjunctions and particles, the analysis aims to trace the possible development of jako from conjunction to a tag-like particle that can be exploited for mobilizing affiliative responses. Traditionally, jako has been described as conjunction used for comparing two elements or for providing a specification of a first element [“X like Y”]. In spoken Czech, however, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Combining different activities in family-style group care: How Professional Foster Parents show listenership towards adolescents during dinner related activities.Martine Noordegraaf & Ellen Schep - 2022 - Discourse Studies 24 (3):350-370.
    This research focuses on dinner conversations in family-style group care. Children, who cannot live with their biological families anymore, are given shelter in these family-style group care settings. For the development of an attachment relationship between children and their Professional Foster Parents, it is important that the children feel that they are listened to in order to get an affective and intimate relationship with the parents. In this conversation-analytic research we analysed PFPs’ involvement in multiple activities simultaneously, namely listening and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Highlighted moves within an action: segmented talk in Japanese conversation.Emi Morita - 2008 - Discourse Studies 10 (4):517-541.
    Japanese conversational data reveal that Japanese speakers produce, and recipients orient to, smaller units of talk than what the conventional notion of a `turn constructional unit' represents. Unlike TCUs, such units may be grammatically, prosodically and pragmatically incomplete and may happen on the sub-phrasal level of discourse, as Japanese conversationalists prosodically break up even a single semantic constituent with the insertion of an interactional particle. In this article, I give numerous examples of how such practices of separating a segment of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The projectability of turn constructional units and the role of prediction in listening.Anthony J. Liddicoat - 2004 - Discourse Studies 6 (4):449-469.
    In conversation analysis, prediction, in the form of the projectability of turn constructional units, is a central feature of the ways in which talk is structured in interaction. As such, prediction is an important part of what recipients do when listening to talk in progress. This article will examine a number of instances of collaborative talk in English and French in which both participants produce similar talk at roughly the same time. It will be shown that prediction is being exercised (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Tacit acceptance of compliments after tellings of accomplishment: Contingent management of preferences in Japanese ordinary conversation.Akiko Imamura - 2022 - Discourse Studies 24 (2):206-230.
    This study investigates Japanese compliments produced at a distinct sequential position and how the complimentees treat the compliments. In ordinary conversation, speakers sometimes talk about their accomplishments. Drawing on Conversation Analysis and multimodal interaction analysis, the study demonstrates how telling recipients deploy compliments at the possible completion of such tellings of accomplishment. The analysis also shows how the tellers deal with the complimentary telling responses, taking into consideration the design of tellings and the possibility of engaging in self-praise. The study (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Marking understanding versus receipting information in talk: Achso. and ach in German interaction.Andrea Golato - 2010 - Discourse Studies 12 (2):147-176.
    This conversation analytic study contrasts the German particles ach and achso. and discusses their form, function and interactional trajectory. It extends Golato and Betz’s work on ach and achso. in third positions of repair sequences to other positions and actions, and compares it to work on English oh. Based on the analysis of over 200 instances, I argue that ach so. is used to explicitly mark understanding of a prior action or of the import of the speaker’s own actions while (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • ‘Let’s check-in with our tummies’: Orienting to feelings-talk in group supervision for psychotherapy counsellors.Alison Dart & Ian Hutchby - 2018 - Discourse Studies 20 (5):598-612.
    This article examines a particular kind of business-opening activity found in a specific, and little analysed, type of institutional group meeting: group supervision for psychotherapeutic counsellors. The data consist of a particular set of activities that occur in the initial stages of these meetings, which are neither the kind of pre-meeting talk identified by previous research on interaction in meetings, nor specifically the business of group supervision itself. This phase, referred to as the ‘check-in’, functions as an interim stage between (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pragmatic Competence Injustice.Manuel Padilla Cruz - 2018 - Social Epistemology 32 (3):143-163.
    When engaging in verbal communication, we do not simply use language to dispense information, but also to perform a plethora of actions, some of which depend on conventionalised, recurrent linguistic structures. Additionally, we must be skilled enough to arrive at the speaker’s intended meaning. However, speakers’ performance may deviate from certain habits and expectations concerning the way of speaking or accomplishing actions, while various factors may hinder comprehension, which may give rise to misappraisals of their respective abilities and capacities as (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Contributing to Discourse.Herbert H. Clark & Edward F. Schaefer - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (2):259-294.
    For people to contribute to discourse, they must do more than utter the right sentence at the right time. The basic requirement is that they add to their common ground in an orderly way. To do this, we argue, they try to establish for each utterance the mutual belief that the addressees have understood what the speaker meant well enough for current purposes. This is accomplished by the collective actions of the current contributor and his or her partners, and these (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   79 citations  
  • Navigating joint projects with dialogue.Adrian Bangerter & Herbert H. Clark - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (2):195-225.
    Dialogue has its origins in joint activities, which it serves to coordinate. Joint activities, in turn, usually emerge in hierarchically nested projects and subprojects. We propose that participants use dialogue to coordinate two kinds of transitions in these joint projects: vertical transitions, or entering and exiting joint projects; and horizontal transitions, or continuing within joint projects. The participants help signal these transitions with project markers, words such as uh-huh, m-hm, yeah, okay, or all right. These words have been studied mainly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Repair: The Interface Between Interaction and Cognition.Saul Albert & J. P. de Ruiter - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 10 (2):279-313.
    Albert and De Ruiter provide an introduction to the Conversation Analytic approach to ‘repair’: the ways in which people detect and deal with troubles in speaking, hearing and understanding in conversation. They explain the basic turn‐taking structures involved, provide examples, explain recent developments in the field and highlight some important points of contact and contrast with work in the Cognitive Sciences.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations