Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Coordinate transformations in orofacial movements.D. J. Ostry, J. R. Flanagan & L. E. Sergio - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):348-349.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Command invariants and the frame of reference for human movement.David J. Ostry, Rafael Laboissière & Paul L. Gribble - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):770-772.
    We describe a solution to the redundancy problem related to that proposed in Feldman & Levin's target article. We suggest that the system may use a fixed mapping between commands organized at the level of degrees of freedom and commands to individual muscles. This proposal eliminates the need to maintain an explicit representation of musculoskeletalgeometry in planning movements.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Interneurons as backseat drivers and the elusive control variable.T. Richard Nichols - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):772-773.
    It is proposed here that the spinal network of proprioceptive feedback from length and force receptors constitutes the mechanism underlying the coordination of activation thresholds for muscles acting about the same and neighboring joints. For the most part, these circuits come between motoneurons and supraspinal signals, invalidating the idea that the activation thresholds constitute control variables for the motor system.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Frames of reference and normal movement.Karl M. Newell & Steven Morrison - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):83-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Coordinate transformations or dynamic models?Peter D. Neilson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):348-348.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Rehabilitation promotes functional movement in atypical populations.Meg Morris, Thomas Matyas, Robert Iansek & Ross Cunnington - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):82-83.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motor simulation.Adam Morton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):215-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Kinematic invariances and body schema.Pietro Morasso & Vittorio Sanguineti - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):769-770.
    Generalizing the notion that muscles are positional frames of reference, a high-dimensional muscle space is defined for multi-muscle systems with an embedded low-dimensional motor manifold of functional articulators. A central representation of such a manifold is proposed as computational body schema. The example of the jaw-tongue system is presented, discussing the relation of functional articulators with kinematic invariances and control problems.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are motor images based on kinestheticvisual matching?Robert W. Mitchell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):214-215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Visually guided action and the “need to know”.A. David Milner, David P. Carey & Monika Harvey - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):213-214.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can the λ model benefit from understanding human adaptation in weightlessness(and vice versa)?P. Vernon McDonald - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):768-768.
    Parameters of the lambda model seem tightly linked to certain characteristics of human performance influenced by weightlessness. This commentary suggests that there is a valuable opportunity to probe the lambda model using the changed environment experienced during space flight. The likely benefits are a better model and a better understanding ofthe consequences of weightlessness for human performance.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Invariants of the second transformation expressed in activation ranges.Gin McCollum - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):346-348.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What are “normal movements” in any population?R. S. W. Masters & R. C. J. Polman - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):81-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Coordinate transformations: Some basic questions.Lina L. E. Massone - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):345-346.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Adaptive rationality and identifiability of psychological processes.Dominic W. Massaro & Daniel Friedman - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):499-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Theories need data and patients need treatment: Where's the beef?Mathew T. Martin-Iverson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):80-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The issue of motor equivalence.R. G. Marteniuk & H. Carnahan - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):356-357.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What can we expect from models of motor control?Gerald E. Loeb - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):767-768.
    The lambda model of servocontrol seems superior to the alpha model in terms of dealing with the mechanical complexities of nonlinear and multiarticular muscles. Both, however, can be trivialized by noting that the “control variable” may simply be the sum of descending influences at propriospinal interneurons in the case of the lambda model or in the muscles themselves in the case of the alpha model. The notion that the brain explicitly computes output in terms of any such control variables may (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Frameworks on shifting sands.R. Lngvaldsen & H. T. A. Whiting - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):764-765.
    Feldman and Levin present a model for movement control in which the system is said to seek equilibrium points, active movement being produced by shifting frames of reference in space. It is argued that whatever merit this model might have is limited to an understanding of “the how” and not “the why” we move. In this way the authors seem to be forced into a dualistic position leaving the upper level of the proposed control hierarchy “floating.”.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The λ model for motor control: More than meets the eye.Mindy F. Levin & Anatol G. Feldman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):786-806.
    Understanding of the λ model has greatly increased in recent years as evidenced by most of the commentaries. Some commentators underscored the potential of the model to integrate aspects of different sensorimotor systems in the production of movement. Other commentators focused on not-yet-fully-developed parts of the model. A few persisted in misunderstanding some of its basic concepts, and on these grounds they reject it. In responding to commentaries we continue to elaborate on some fundamental points of the model, especially control (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Should stereotypic movement synergies in hemiparetic patients be considered adaptive?Mindy F. Levin - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):79-80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What does body configuration in microgravity tell us about the contribution of intra- and extrapersonal frames of reference for motor control?F. Lestienne, M. Ghafouri & F. Thullier - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):766-767.
    The authors report that the reorganization of body configuration during weightlessness is based on an intrapersonal frame of reference such as the configuration of the support surface and the position of the body's center of gravity. These results stress the importance of “knowledge” of the state of internal geometric structures, which cannot be directly signalled by specific receptors responsible for direct dialogue with the physical external world.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are there multiple movement strategies?Robert G. Lee - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):356-356.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What are “normal movements” in atypical populations?Mark L. Latash & J. Greg Anson - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):55-68.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Equilibrium-point control? Yes! Deterministic mechanisms of control? No!Mark L. Latash - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):765-766.
    The equilibrium-point hypothesis (the λ-model) is superior to all other models of single-joint control and provides deep insights into the mechanisms of control of multi-joint movements. Attempts at associating control variables with neurophysiological variables look confusing rather than promising. Probabilistic mechanisms may play an important role in movement generation in redundant systems.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern.Ning Lan & Patrick E. Crago - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):769-771.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Coordinate transformations in postural control.Francesco Lacquaniti - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):345-345.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Network relaxation as biological computation.Hon C. Kwan, Tet H. Yeap, Donald Barrett & Bai C. Jiang - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):354-356.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The concept of “normal” movement and its consequences for therapy.Jürgen Konczak & Johannes Dichgans - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):79-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What makes a population atypical–priorities or constraints?Roberta L. Klatzky - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):78-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the relation between motor imagery and visual imagery.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):212-213.
    Jeannerod's target article describes support, through empirical and neurological findings, for the intriguing idea of motor imagery, a form of representation hypothesized to have levels of functional equivalence with motor preparation, while being consciously accessible. Jeannerod suggests that the subjectively accessible content of motor imagery allows it to be distinguished from motor preparation, which is unconscious. Motor imagery is distinguished from visual imagery in terms of content. Motor images are kinesthetic in nature; they are parametrized by variables such as force (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Frames of reference interact and are task-dependent.Bruce A. Kay - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):765-765.
    The problem for the CNS in any particular movement task is to coordinate the various frames of reference appropriate to the task. Control variables are determined by this coordination. The coordination problem varies greatly from task to task, and so no single set of control variables is likely to account for a broad range of movement tasks.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anticipatory postural mechanisms: Some evidence and methodological implications.Tatsuya Kasai - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):77-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Limitations on the what reaching can tell us about sensorimotor transformations.Michael Kalish - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):344-344.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Synergy versus schema.P. C. Kainen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):212-212.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery.Marc Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):187-202.
    This paper concerns how motor actions are neurally represented and coded. Action planning and motor preparation can be studied using a specific type of representational activity, motor imagery. A close functional equivalence between motor imagery and motor preparation is suggested by the positive effects of imagining movements on motor learning, the similarity between the neural structures involved, and the similar physiological correlates observed in both imaging and preparing. The content of motor representations can be inferred from motor images at a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   341 citations  
  • Motor representations and reality.M. Jeannerod - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):229-245.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • What is coded in parietal representations?Ray Jackendoff & Barbara Landau - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):211-212.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motor memory – a memory of the future.David H. Ingvar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Probing the “Achilles' heel” of rational analysis.Keith J. Holyoak - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):498-499.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Information decay during response delay.Dennis H. Holding - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):343-344.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “Constraint” versus “choice” in preferred movement patterns.Kenneth G. Holt - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):76-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The unobservability of central commands: Why testing hypotheses is so difficult.Antony Hodgson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):763-764.
    The experiments Feldman and Levin suggest do not definitively test their proposed solution to the problem of selecting muscle activations. Their test of the movement directions that elicit EMG activity can be interpreted without regard to the form of the central commands, and their fast elbow flexion test is based on a forward computation that obscures the insensitivity of the predicted trajectory to the details of the putative commands.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Physical modeling applies to physiology, too.Vincent Hayward - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):342-343.
    A physical model was utilized to show that the neural system can memorize a target position and is able to cause motor and sensory events that move the arm to a target with more accuracy. However, this cannot indicate in which coordinates the necessary computations are carried out. Turning off the lights causes the error to increase which is accomplished by cutting off one feedback path. The geometrical properties of arm kinematics and the properties of the kinesthetic and visual sensorial (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do control variables exist?Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos & William H. Warren - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):762-762.
    We argue that the concept of a control variable (CV) as described by Feldman and Levin needs to be revised because it does not account for the influence of sensory feedback from the periphery. We provide evidence from the realm of rhythmic movements that sensory feedback can permanently alter the frequency and phase of a centrally generated rhythm.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational analysis and the Lens model.Reid Hastie & Kenneth R. Hammond - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):498-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is stiffness the mainspring of posture and movement?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):756-758.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Are errors in final position destined before the movement begins?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):341-342.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Is λ an appropriate control variable for locomotion?Thomas M. Hamm & Zong-Sheng Han - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):761-762.
    The lambda model predicts that the command received by each motor nucleus during locomotion is specific for the joint at which its muscle acts and is independent of external conditions. However, investigation of the commands received by motor nuclei during fictive locomotion and of the sensitivity of these commands to feedback from the limb during locomotion indicates that neither condition is satisfied.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery and mental practice.Mark Hallett, Jordan Fieldman, Leonardo G. Cohen, Norihiro Sadato & Alvaro Pascual-Leone - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):210-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations