Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. High level processing scope in spoken sentence production.Mark Smith & Linda Wheeldon - 1999 - Cognition 73 (3):205-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • A theory of lexical access in speech production.Willem J. M. Levelt, Ardi Roelofs & Antje S. Meyer - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (1):1-38.
    Preparing words in speech production is normally a fast and accurate process. We generate them two or three per second in fluent conversation; and overtly naming a clear picture of an object can easily be initiated within 600 msec after picture onset. The underlying process, however, is exceedingly complex. The theory reviewed in this target article analyzes this process as staged and feedforward. After a first stage of conceptual preparation, word generation proceeds through lexical selection, morphological and phonological encoding, phonetic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   268 citations  
  • Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations.Willem J. M. Levelt - 1992 - Cognition 42 (1-3):1-22.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • Verbal hallucinations and speech disorganization in schizophrenia: A further look at the evidence.Martin Harrow, Joanne T. Marengo & Ann Ragin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):526-526.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Verbal hallucinations and language production processes in schizophrenia.Ralph E. Hoffman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):503-517.
    How is it that many schizophrenics identify certain instances of verbal imagery as hallucinatory? Most investigators have assumed that alterations in sensory features of imagery explain this. This approach, however, has not yielded a definitive picture of the nature of verbal hallucinations. An alternative perspective suggests itself if one allows the possibility that the nonself quality of hallucinations is inferred on the basis of the experience of unintendedness that accompanies imagery production. Information-processing models of “intentional” cognitive processes call for abstract (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Hallucinations and contextually generated interpretations.Nicholas P. Spanos - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):533-534.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rationality, Reasoning Well, and Extramental Props.Wade Munroe - 2019 - Res Philosophica 96 (2):175-198.
    Recently, a cottage industry has formed with the expressed intent of analyzing the nature of personal-level reasoning and inference. The dominant position in the extant philosophical literature is that reasoning consists in rule-governed operations over propositional attitudes. In addition, it is widely assumed that our attitude updating procedures are purely cognitive. Any non-cognitive activity performed in service of updating our attitudes is external to the updating process—at least in terms of rational evaluation. In this paper, I argue that whether one (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Reality and control.James Deese - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):521-522.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intentionality and autonomy of verbal imagery in altered states of consciousness.David F. Marks - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):529-530.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Paragrammatisms: Syntactic disturbance or breakdown of control?Trevor A. Harley - 1990 - Cognition 34 (1):85-91.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Intended versus intentional action.Myles Brand - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):520-521.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The diversity of the schizophrenias.Raymond Faber - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):522-522.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A noisy-channel approach to depth-charge illusions.Yuhan Zhang, Rachel Ryskin & Edward Gibson - 2023 - Cognition 232 (C):105346.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What it takes to make a word.Wade Munroe - 2022 - Synthese 200 (4):1-30.
    Consider the following object, where, depending on how you are viewing this paper, the object may be a series of ink markings, a portion of a matrix of pixels through or from which light is emitted, etc.,augeLet’s call the object ‘Shape’. Is Shape a word token? If so, what word type is it a token of? Given how words are traditionally individuated, the Spanish, “auge”—meaning, apogee or peak—the French, “auge”—meaning, basin or bowl—and the German, “auge”—meaning, eye, are different words. So, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers.Gary S. Dell, Myrna F. Schwartz, Nadine Martin, Eleanor M. Saffran & Deborah A. Gagnon - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (4):801-838.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Contrasting effects of phonological priming in aphasic word production.Carolyn E. Wilshire & Eleanor M. Saffran - 2005 - Cognition 95 (1):31-71.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Disorders of lexical selection.Merrill Garrett - 1992 - Cognition 42 (1-3):143-180.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Investigation of phonological encoding through speech error analyses: Achievements, limitations, and alternatives.Antje S. Meyer - 1992 - Cognition 42 (1-3):181-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking.Ardi Roelofs - 1992 - Cognition 42 (1-3):107-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   97 citations  
  • The sequential cuing effect in speech production.Christine A. Sevald & Gary S. Dell - 1994 - Cognition 53 (2):91-127.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Verbal hallucinations, unintendedness, and the validity of the schizophrenia diagnosis.R. P. Bentall & P. D. Slade - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):519-520.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Connectionist Models of Language Production: Lexical Access and Grammatical Encoding.Gary S. Dell, Franklin Chang & Zenzi M. Griffin - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):517-542.
    Theories of language production have long been expressed as connectionist models. We outline the issues and challenges that must be addressed by connectionist models of lexical access and grammatical encoding, and review three recent models. The models illustrate the value of an interactive activation approach to lexical access in production, the need for sequential output in both phonological and grammatical encoding, and the potential for accounting for structural effects on errors and structural priming from learning.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Positive feedback in hierarchical connectionist models: Applications to language production.Gary S. Dell - 1985 - Cognitive Science 9 (1):3-23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Integrating experiential and distributional data to learn semantic representations.Mark Andrews, Gabriella Vigliocco & David Vinson - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (3):463-498.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • From Monroe to Moreau: An analysis of face naming errors.Serge Brédart & Tim Valentine - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):187-223.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Stages of lexical access in language production.Gary S. Dell & Padraig G. O'Seaghdha - 1992 - Cognition 42 (1-3):287-314.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   54 citations  
  • A three-component analysis of Hoffman's model of verbal hallucinations.Heidelinde Allen - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):518-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Hallucination, rationalization, and response set.Steven Schwartz - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):532-533.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Who may I say is calling?Kathleen A. Akins & Daniel C. Dennett - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):517-518.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Speech errors and hallucinations in schizophrenia – no difference?Trevor A. Harley - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):525-526.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming.Willem J. Levelt, Herbert Schriefers, Dirk Vorberg & Antje S. Meyer - 1991 - Psychological Review 98 (1):122-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Verbal hallucinations and information processing.Bjørn Rishovd Rund - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):531-532.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • Learning to divide the labor: an account of deficits in light and heavy verb production.Jean K. Gordon & Gary S. Dell - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (1):1-40.
    Theories of sentence production that involve a convergence of activation from conceptual‐semantic and syntactic‐sequential units inspired a connectionist model that was trained to produce simple sentences. The model used a learning algorithm that resulted in a sharing of responsibility (or “division of labor”) between syntactic and semantic inputs for lexical activation according to their predictive power. Semantically rich, or “heavy”, verbs in the model came to rely on semantic cues more than on syntactic cues, whereas semantically impoverished, or “light”, verbs (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • When is an image hallucinatory?Graham F. Reed - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):530-531.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Image or neural coding of inner speech and agency?Gail Zivin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):534-535.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Does cognitive neuropsychology have a future?J. T. L. Wilson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):456-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production.Ardi Roelofs - 1997 - Cognition 64 (3):249-284.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Verbal hallucinations also occur in normals.Thomas B. Posey - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):530-530.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Lexical access and discourse planning: Bottom-up interference or top-down control troubles?Wendy G. Lehnert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):528-529.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • An Incremental Procedural Grammar for Sentence Formulation.Gerard Kempen & Edward Hoenkamp - 1987 - Cognitive Science 11 (2):201-258.
    This paper presents a theory of the syntactic aspects of human sentence production. An important characteristic of unprepared speech is that overt pronunciation of a sentence can be initiated before the speaker has completely worked out the meaning content he or she is going to express in that sentence. Apparently, the speaker is able to build up a syntactically coherent utterance out of a series of syntactic fragments each rendering a new part of the meaning content. This incremental, left‐to‐right mode (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Distinctiveness, unintendedness, location, and nonself attribution of verbal hallucinations.John Junginger - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):527-528.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hearing voices and the bicameral mind.Julian Jaynes - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):526-527.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What can schizophrenic “voices” tell us?Ralph E. Hoffman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):535-548.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Teleology and agency in speech production.Robert M. Gordon - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):525-525.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Arousal and the disruption of language production processes in schizophrenia.Per F. Gjerde - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):524-524.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Auditory hallucinations, inner speech, and the dominant hemisphere.Pierre Flor-Henry - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):523-524.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Language process and hallucination phenomenology.Murray Alpert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):518-519.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Cascading activation in phonological planning and articulation: Evidence from spontaneous speech errors.John Alderete, Melissa Baese-Berk, Keith Leung & Matthew Goldrick - 2021 - Cognition 210 (C):104577.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark