Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Accuracy-based measures provide a better measure of sequence learning than reaction time-based measures.Kristi Urry, Nicholas R. Burns & Irina Baetu - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:153321.
    The Serial Reaction Time Task (SRTT) was designed to measure motor sequence learning and is widely used in many fields in cognitive science and neuroscience. However, the common performance measures derived from SRTT—reaction time (RT) difference scores—may not provide valid measures of sequence learning. This is because RT-difference scores may be subject to floor effects and otherwise not sufficiently reflective of learning. A ratio RT measure might minimize floor effects. Furthermore, measures derived from predictive accuracy may provide a better assessment (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A new approach to the formulation and testing of learning models.Joseph F. Hanna - 1966 - Synthese 16 (3-4):344 - 380.
    It is argued that current attempts to model human learning behavior commonly fail on one of two counts: either the model assumptions are artificially restricted so as to permit the application of mathematical techniques in deriving their consequences, or else the required complex assumptions are imbedded in computer programs whose technical details obscure the theoretical content of the model. The first failing is characteristic of so-called mathematical models of learning, while the second is characteristic of computer simulation models. An approach (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • (1 other version)How do we know what we are doing? Time, intention and awareness of action.Jean-Christophe Sarrazin, Axel Cleeremans & Patrick Haggard - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3):602-615.
    Time is a fundamental dimension of consciousness. Many studies of the “sense of agency” have investigated whether we attribute actions to ourselves based on a conscious experience of intention occurring prior to action, or based on a reconstruction after the action itself has occurred. Here, we ask the same question about a lower level aspect of action experience, namely awareness of the detailed spatial form of a simple movement. Subjects reached for a target, which unpredictably jumped to the side on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Predicting Outcomes in a Sequence of Binary Events: Belief Updating and Gambler's Fallacy Reasoning.Kariyushi Rao & Reid Hastie - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (1):e13211.
    Beliefs like the Gambler's Fallacy and the Hot Hand have interested cognitive scientists, economists, and philosophers for centuries. We propose that these judgment patterns arise from the observer's mental models of the sequence-generating mechanism, moderated by the strength of belief in an a priori base rate. In six behavioral experiments, participants observed one of three mechanisms generating sequences of eight binary events: a random mechanical device, an intentional goal-directed actor, and a financial market. We systematically manipulated participants’ beliefs about the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)How do we know what we are doing? Time, intention and awareness of action☆.Jean-Christophe Sarrazin, Axel Cleeremans & Patrick Haggard - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3):602-615.
    Time is a fundamental dimension of consciousness. Many studies of the “sense of agency” have investigated whether we attribute actions to ourselves based on a conscious experience of intention occurring prior to action, or based on a reconstruction after the action itself has occurred. Here, we ask the same question about a lower level aspect of action experience, namely awareness of the detailed spatial form of a simple movement. Subjects reached for a target, which unpredictably jumped to the side on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Making sense of randomness: Implicit encoding as a basis for judgment.Ruma Falk & Clifford Konold - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (2):301-318.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • The Alternative Omen Effect: Illusory negative correlation between the outcomes of choice options.Déborah Marciano-Romm, Assaf Romm, Sacha Bourgeois-Gironde & Leon Y. Deouell - 2016 - Cognition 146:324-338.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Explaining the gambler's fallacy: Testing a gestalt explanation versus the “law of small numbers”.Christopher J. R. Roney & Natalie Sansone - 2015 - Thinking and Reasoning 21 (2):193-205.
    The present study tests a gestalt explanation for the gambler's fallacy which posits that runs in random events will be expected to reverse only when the run is open or ongoing. This is contrasted with the law of small numbers explanation suggesting that people expect random outcomes to balance out generally. Sixty-one university students placed hypothetical guesses and bets on a series of coin tosses. Either heads or tails were dominant . In a closed run condition the run ended prior (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Studying short-term storage of expectancies with reaction time.E. Scott Geller - 1982 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 19 (1):3-6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Direct Associations or Internal Transformations? Exploring the Mechanisms Underlying Sequential Learning Behavior.Todd M. Gureckis & Bradley C. Love - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (1):10-50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The production and perception of randomness.Raymond S. Nickerson - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (2):330-357.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations