- Téléologie et fonctions en biologie. Une approche non causale des explications téléofonctionnelles.Alberto Molina Pérez - 2017 - Dissertation, Universidad Autónoma de Madriddetails
|
|
Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: when does language become a tool for categorization?Thierry Nazzi & Alison Gopnik - 2001 - Cognition 80 (3):B11-B20.details
|
|
Artifact Categorization. Trends and Problems.Massimiliano Carrara & Daria Mingardo - 2013 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 4 (3):351-373.details
|
|
Cognitive linguistics as a methodological paradigm.Prof A. V. Kravchenko - 2002 - In [Book Chapter].details
|
|
Information Sources for Noun Learning.Edward Kako - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (2):223-260.details
|
|
Function is not the sum of an object’s parts.Krista Casler - 2018 - Thinking and Reasoning 25 (3):300-323.details
|
|
(1 other version)Was it designed to do that? Children’s focus on intended function in their conceptualization of artifacts.Yvonne M. Asher & Deborah G. Kemler Nelson - 2008 - Cognition 106 (1):474-483.details
|
|
The role of functional information for infant categorization.Birgit Träuble & Sabina Pauen - 2007 - Cognition 105 (2):362-379.details
|
|
(1 other version)Categorical structure among shared features in networks of early-learned nouns.Thomas T. Hills, Mounir Maouene, Josita Maouene, Adam Sheya & Linda Smith - 2009 - Cognition 112 (3):381-396.details
|
|
Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words.Daniel N. Bub, Michael E. J. Masson & George S. Cree - 2008 - Cognition 106 (1):27-58.details
|
|
The role of historical intuitions in children's and adults' naming of artifacts.Grant Gutheil, Paul Bloom, Nohemy Valderrama & Rebecca Freedman - 2004 - Cognition 91 (1):23-42.details
|
|
Inference and coherence in causal-based artifact categorization.Guillermo Puebla & Sergio E. Chaigneau - 2014 - Cognition 130 (1):50-65.details
|
|
Creators’ intentions bias judgments of function independently from causal inferences.Sergio E. Chaigneau, Ramón D. Castillo & Luis Martínez - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):123-132.details
|
|
From the Lexicon to Expectations About Kinds: A Role for Associative Learning.Eliana Colunga & Linda B. Smith - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (2):347-382.details
|
|
Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge.Lucas P. Butler & Ellen M. Markman - 2014 - Cognition 130 (1):116-127.details
|
|
The emerging causal understanding of institutional objects.Alexander Noyes, Frank C. Keil & Yarrow Dunham - 2018 - Cognition 170 (C):83-87.details
|
|
Reasoning about artifacts at 24 months: The developing teleo-functional stance.Krista Casler & Deborah Kelemen - 2007 - Cognition 103 (1):120-130.details
|
|
Becoming status conscious: Children's appreciation of social reality.Charles Kalish - 2005 - Philosophical Explorations 8 (3):245 – 263.details
|
|
Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis.Ashley de Marchena, Inge-Marie Eigsti, Amanda Worek, Kim Emiko Ono & Jesse Snedeker - 2011 - Cognition 119 (1):96-113.details
|
|
Priming infants to attend to color and pattern information in an individuation task.Teresa Wilcox & Catherine Chapa - 2004 - Cognition 90 (3):265-302.details
|
|
How children come to (not) detect and apply multiple functions for objects: Rethinking perseveration and functional fixedness.Maria Pflüger, David Buttelmann & Birgit Elsner - 2024 - Cognition 251 (C):105902.details
|
|
Word learning is 'smart': evidence that conceptual information affects preschoolers' extension of novel words.A. Booth - 2002 - Cognition 84 (1):B11-B22.details
|
|