Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Causal model progressions as a foundation for intelligent learning environments.Barbara Y. White & John R. Frederiksen - 1990 - Artificial Intelligence 42 (1):99-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • “What if…”: The Use of Conceptual Simulations in Scientific Reasoning.Susan Bell Trickett & J. Gregory Trafton - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (5):843-875.
    The term conceptual simulation refers to a type of everyday reasoning strategy commonly called “what if” reasoning. It has been suggested in a number of contexts that this type of reasoning plays an important role in scientific discovery; however, little direct evidence exists to support this claim. This article proposes that conceptual simulation is likely to be used in situations of informational uncertainty, and may be used to help scientists resolve that uncertainty. We conducted two studies to investigate the relationship (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Shuttling Between Depictive Models and Abstract Rules: Induction and Fallback.Daniel L. Schwartz & John B. Black - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (4):457-497.
    A productive way to think about imagistic mental models of physical systems is as though they were sources of quasi‐empirical evidence. People depict or imagine events at those points in time when they would experiment with the world if possible. Moreover, just as they would do when observing the world, people induce patterns of behavior from the results depicted in their imaginations. These resulting patterns of behavior can then be cast into symbolic rules to simplify thinking about future problems and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Modeling How, When, and What Is Learned in a Simple Fault‐Finding Task.Frank E. Ritter & Peter A. Bibby - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (5):862-892.
    We have developed a process model that learns in multiple ways while finding faults in a simple control panel device. The model predicts human participants' learning through its own learning. The model's performance was systematically compared to human learning data, including the time course and specific sequence of learned behaviors. These comparisons show that the model accounts very well for measures such as problem‐solving strategy, the relative difficulty of faults, and average fault‐finding time. More important, because the model learns and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Role of a Mental Model in Learning to Operate a Device.David E. Kieras & Susan Bovair - 1984 - Cognitive Science 8 (3):255-273.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Understanding and Resolving Failures in Human-Robot Interaction: Literature Review and Model Development.Shanee Honig & Tal Oron-Gilad - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Arrows in Comprehending and Producing Mechanical Diagrams.Julie Heiser & Barbara Tversky - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (3):581-592.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Instruction and Practice in Learning to use a Device.Peter A. Bibby & Stephen J. Payne - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (4):539-578.
    We explore the extent to which Anderson's (1987) theory of knowledge compilation can account for the relationship between instructions and practice in learning to use a simple device. Bibby and Payne (1993) reported experimental support for knowledge compilation in this domain. This article replicates the finding of a performance cross‐over between instruction type and task type that disappears with practice on the tasks. The research is extended by using verbal protocols to model the strategies of novice and more experienced individuals. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • When scientific models represent.Daniela M. Bailer-Jones - 2003 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 17 (1):59 – 74.
    Scientific models represent aspects of the empirical world. I explore to what extent this representational relationship, given the specific properties of models, can be analysed in terms of propositions to which truth or falsity can be attributed. For example, models frequently entail false propositions despite the fact that they are intended to say something "truthful" about phenomena. I argue that the representational relationship is constituted by model users "agreeing" on the function of a model, on the fit with data and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • Mental Models, Psychology of.J. M. Loomis, R. L. Klatzky, R. G. Golledge & J. G. CicineIli - 1991 - In Stephen Everson (ed.), Psychology (Companions to Ancient Thought: 2). Cambridge University Press. pp. 56-89.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Distributed mental models: Mental models in distributed cognitive systems.Adrian P. Banks & Lynne J. Millward - 2009 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 30 (4):249-266.
    The function of groups as information processors is increasingly being recognised in a number of theories of group cognition. A theme of many of these is an emphasis on sharing cognition. This paper extends current conceptualisations of groups by critiquing the focus on shared cognition and emphasising the distribution of cognition in groups. In particular, it develops an account of the distribution of one cognitive construct, mental models. Mental models have been chosen as a focus because they are used in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark