Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Testing adaptive toolbox models: A Bayesian hierarchical approach.Benjamin Scheibehenne, Jörg Rieskamp & Eric-Jan Wagenmakers - 2013 - Psychological Review 120 (1):39-64.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The cognitive architecture for chaining of two mental operations.Jérôme Sackur & Stanislas Dehaene - 2009 - Cognition 111 (2):187-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • The interoception and imagination loop in hypnotic phenomena.Alain Parra & Arnaud Rey - 2019 - Consciousness and Cognition 73:102765.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Virtual Orientation Overrides Physical Orientation to Define a Reference Frame in Spatial Updating.Qiliang He & Timothy P. McNamara - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A Neural Theory of Visual Attention: Bridging Cognition and Neurophysiology.Claus Bundesen, Thomas Habekost & Søren Kyllingsbæk - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (2):291-328.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • The physics of optimal decision making: A formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks.Rafal Bogacz, Eric Brown, Jeff Moehlis, Philip Holmes & Jonathan D. Cohen - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (4):700-765.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy.Edward Awh, Artem V. Belopolsky & Jan Theeuwes - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (8):437.
    Prominent models of attentional control assert a dichotomy between top-down and bottom-up control, with the former determined by current selection goals and the latter determined by physical salience. This theoretical dichotomy, however, fails to explain a growing number of cases in which neither current goals nor physical salience can account for strong selection biases. For example, equally salient stimuli associated with reward can capture attention, even when this contradicts current selection goals. Thus, although 'top-down' sources of bias are sometimes defined (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   99 citations  
  • In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects. [REVIEW]Julie M. Bugg & Matthew J. C. Crump - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Episodic traces and statistical regularities: Paired associate learning in typical and dyslexic readers.Manon Wyn Jones, Jan-Rouke Kuipers, Sinead Nugent, Angelina Miley & Gary Oppenheim - 2018 - Cognition 177 (C):214-225.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational and mechanistic perspectives on reinforcement learning.Nick Chater - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):350-364.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Task conditions and short-term memory search: two-phase model of STM search.Robert Balas, Edward Nęcka & Jarosław Orzechowski - 2016 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 47 (1):12-20.
    Short-term memory search, as investigated within the Sternberg paradigm, is usually described as exhaustive rather than self-terminated, although the debate concerning these issues is still hot. We report three experiments employing a modified Sternberg paradigm and show that whether STM search is exhaustive or self-terminated depends on task conditions. Specifically, STM search self-terminates as soon as a positive match is found, whereas exhaustive search occurs when the STM content does not contain a searched item. Additionally, we show that task conditions (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the ability to inhibit thought and action: General and special theories of an act of control.Gordon D. Logan, Trisha Van Zandt, Frederick Verbruggen & Eric-Jan Wagenmakers - 2014 - Psychological Review 121 (1):66-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention.Ronald Hübner, Marco Steinhauser & Carola Lehle - 2010 - Psychological Review 117 (3):759-784.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • On the interdependence of cognition and emotion.Justin Storbeck & Gerald L. Clore - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1212-1237.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Attentional influences on affective priming: Does categorisation influence spontaneous evaluations of multiply categorisable objects?Bertram Gawronski, William A. Cunningham, Etienne P. LeBel & Roland Deutsch - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (6):1008-1025.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Rumination and inhibition in task switching: no evidence for an association.James A. Grange - 2023 - Cognition and Emotion 37 (1):162-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Prior target locations attract overt attention during search.Travis N. Talcott & Nicholas Gaspelin - 2020 - Cognition 201:104282.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A dynamic stimulus-driven model of signal detection.Brandon M. Turner, Trisha Van Zandt & Scott Brown - 2011 - Psychological Review 118 (4):583-613.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Task representation in individual and joint settings.Wolfgang Prinz - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Exploiting Multiple Sources of Information in Learning an Artificial Language: Human Data and Modeling.Pierre Perruchet & Barbara Tillmann - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (2):255-285.
    This study investigates the joint influences of three factors on the discovery of new word‐like units in a continuous artificial speech stream: the statistical structure of the ongoing input, the initial word‐likeness of parts of the speech flow, and the contextual information provided by the earlier emergence of other word‐like units. Results of an experiment conducted with adult participants show that these sources of information have strong and interactive influences on word discovery. The authors then examine the ability of different (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Identifying bottom-up and top-down components of attentional weight by experimental analysis and computational modeling.Maria Nordfang, Mads Dyrholm & Claus Bundesen - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (2):510.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Where are nature's joints? Finding the mechanisms underlying categorization.Arthur B. Markman - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2-3):220-221.
    Machery argues that concepts are too heterogeneous to be a natural kind. I argue that the book does not go far enough. Theories of concepts assume that the task of categorizing warrants a unique set of cognitive constructs. Instead, cognitive science must look across tasks to find a fundamental set of cognitive mechanisms.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Temporal Distinctiveness in Task Switching: Assessing the Mixture-Distribution Assumption.James A. Grange - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Weighting Mechanisms Within and Across Modalities.Thomas Töllner - unknown
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark