Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Concrete magnitudes: From numbers to time.Christine Falter, Valdas Noreika, Julian Kiverstein & Bruno Mölder - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3-4):335-336.
    Cohen Kadosh & Walsh (CK&W) present convincing evidence indicating the existence of notation-specific numerical representations in parietal cortex. We suggest that the same conclusions can be drawn for a particular type of numerical representation: the representation of time. Notation-dependent representations need not be limited to number but may also be extended to other magnitude-related contents processed in parietal cortex (Walsh 2003).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On Adaptation, Maximization, and Reinforcement Learning Among Cognitive Strategies.Ido Erev & Greg Barron - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (4):912-931.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Implicit practical learning.Elizabeth Ennen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):404-405.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The retention of automatically and effortfully encoded stimulus attributes.Norman R. Ellis & Timothy C. Rickard - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (4):299-302.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Wheelchair Control in a Virtual Environment by Healthy Participants Using a P300-BCI Based on Tactile Stimulation: Training Effects and Usability.Matthias Eidel & Andrea Kübler - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Creatures of habit : a multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects.Tobias Egner - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • The influence of intention on masked priming: A study with semantic classification of words.Doris Eckstein & Walter J. Perrig - 2007 - Cognition 104 (2):345-376.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Compatibility Between Physical Stimulus Size – Spatial Position and False Recognitions.Seda Dural, Birce B. Burhanoǧlu, Nilsu Ekinci, Emre Gürbüz, İdil U. Akın, Seda Can & Hakan Çetinkaya - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skill acquisition in music performance: relations between planning and temporal control.Carolyn Drake & Caroline Palmer - 2000 - Cognition 74 (1):1-32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Dissociable definitions of consciousness.Zoltán Dienes & Josef Perner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):403-404.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Dynamic Network Approach to the Study of Syntax.Holger Diessel - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Usage-based linguists and psychologists have produced a large body of empirical results suggesting that linguistic structure is derived from language use. However, while researchers agree that these results characterize grammar as an emergent phenomenon, there is no consensus among usage-based scholars as to how the various results can be explained and integrated into an explicit theory or model. Building on network theory, the current paper outlines a structured network approach to the study of grammar in which the core concepts of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Automatization through Practice: The Opportunistic‐Stopping Phenomenon Called into Question.Jasinta D. M. Dewi, Jeanne Bagnoud & Catherine Thevenot - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (12):e13074.
    As a theory of skill acquisition, the instance theory of automatization posits that, after a period of training, algorithm‐based performance is replaced by retrieval‐based performance. This theory has been tested using alphabet‐arithmetic verification tasks (e.g., is A + 4 = E?), in which the equations are necessarily solved by counting at the beginning of practice but can be solved by memory retrieval after practice. A way to infer individuals’ strategies in this task was supposedly provided by the opportunistic‐stopping phenomenon, according (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive Control: Dynamic, Sustained, and Voluntary Influences.MaryBeth Knight - unknown
    The cost of incongruent stimuli is reduced when conflict is expected. This series of experiments tested whether this improved performance is due to repetition priming or to enhanced cognitive control. Using a paradigm in which Word and Number Stroop alternated every trial, Experiment 1 assessed dynamic trial-to-trial changes. Incongruent trials led to task-specific reduction of conflict (trial n ϩ 2) without cross-task modulation (trial n ϩ 1), but this was fully explained by repetition priming. In contrast, an increased ratio of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Principles for Implicit Learning.Axel Cleeremans - 1997 - In Dianne C. Berry (ed.), How Implicit is Implicit Learning? Oxford University Press.
    Complete URL to this document: http://srsc.ulb.ac.be/axcWWW/93-Principles.html.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Increased practice with 'set'problems hinders performance on the water jar task.Noelle M. Crooks, Nicole M. McNeil, N. Taatgen & H. Van Rijn - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Time-scale dynamics and the development of an embodied cognition.Esther Thelen - 1995 - In Tim van Gelder & Robert Port (eds.), Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. MIT Press. pp. 69--100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • A plea for methodological dualism and multi-explanation framework in psychology.Sam S. Rakover - 2011 - Behavior and Philosophy 39:17-43.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What is consciousness for?Lee Pierson & Monroe Trout - manuscript
    What is Consciousness For? Lee Pierson and Monroe Trout Copyright © 2005 Abstract: The answer to the title question is, in a word, volition. Our hypothesis is that the ultimate adaptive function of consciousness is to make volitional movement possible. All conscious processes exist to subserve that ultimate function. Thus, we believe that all conscious organisms possess at least some volitional capability. Consciousness makes volitional attention possible; volitional attention, in turn, makes volitional movement possible. There is, as far as we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Thinking as a production system.Marsha C. Lovett & John R. Anderson - 2005 - In K. Holyoak & B. Morrison (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Cambridge University Press. pp. 401--429.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Re-Examining the Role of Consistency: The Cornerstone, not Simply an Important Factor.Patrice Terrier - 1998 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 4.
    Despite the important role of the consistency concept in various theoretical frameworks of memory research and its influence on practical investigations it remains unclear as to whether consistency has been firmly grounded as a explanatory factor. Consistency does not determine either a cognitive load or the development of automaticity. However, it does explain the nature of empirical facts that are subsumed by these terms. Consistency is not a psychological factor involved in many important and highly related topics of consciousness research (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation