Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Type Logical Grammar: Categorial Logic of Signs.G. V. Morrill - 2012 - Dordrecht, Netherland: Springer Verlag.
    This book sets out the foundations, methodology, and practice of a formal framework for the description of language. The approach embraces the trends of lexicalism and compositional semantics in computational linguistics, and theoretical linguistics more broadly, by developing categorial grammar into a powerful and extendable logic of signs. Taking Montague Grammar as its point of departure, the book explains how integration of methods from philosophy (logical semantics), computer science (type theory), linguistics (categorial grammar) and meta-mathematics (mathematical logic ) provides a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • The Linguistic Subversion of Mental Representation.Whit Schonbein - 2012 - Minds and Machines 22 (3):235-262.
    Embedded and embodied approaches to cognition urge that (1) complicated internal representations may be avoided by letting features of the environment drive behavior, and (2) environmental structures can play an enabling role in cognition, allowing prior cognitive processes to solve novel tasks. Such approaches are thus in a natural position to oppose the ‘thesis of linguistic structuring’: The claim that the ability to use language results in a wholesale recapitulation of linguistic structure in onboard mental representation. Prominent examples of researchers (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Multiplicative-Additive Lambek Calculus with Subexponential and Bracket Modalities.Max Kanovich, Stepan Kuznetsov & Andre Scedrov - 2021 - Journal of Logic, Language and Information 30 (1):31-88.
    We give a proof-theoretic and algorithmic complexity analysis for systems introduced by Morrill to serve as the core of the CatLog categorial grammar parser. We consider two recent versions of Morrill’s calculi, and focus on their fragments including multiplicative (Lambek) connectives, additive conjunction and disjunction, brackets and bracket modalities, and the! subexponential modality. For both systems, we resolve issues connected with the cut rule and provide necessary modifications, after which we prove admissibility of cut (cut elimination theorem). We also prove (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Against ellipsis: arguments for the direct licensing of ‘noncanonical’ coordinations.Yusuke Kubota & Robert Levine - 2015 - Linguistics and Philosophy 38 (6):521-576.
    Categorial grammar is well-known for its elegant analysis of coordination enabled by the flexible notion of constituency it entertains. However, to date, no systematic study exists that examines whether this analysis has any obvious empirical advantage over alternative analyses of nonconstituent coordination available in phrase structure-based theories of syntax. This paper attempts precisely such a comparison. We compare the direct constituent coordination analysis of non-canonical coordinations in categorial grammar with an ellipsis-based analysis of the same phenomena in the recent HPSG (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Toward a Connectionist Model of Recursion in Human Linguistic Performance.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (2):157-205.
    Naturally occurring speech contains only a limited amount of complex recursive structure, and this is reflected in the empirically documented difficulties that people experience when processing such structures. We present a connectionist model of human performance in processing recursive language structures. The model is trained on simple artificial languages. We find that the qualitative performance profile of the model matches human behavior, both on the relative difficulty of center‐embedding and cross‐dependency, and between the processing of these complex recursive structures and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   72 citations  
  • Argument or no argument?Geoffrey K. Pullum & Kyle Rawlins - 2007 - Linguistics and Philosophy 30 (2):277 - 287.
    We examine an argument for the non-context-freeness of English that has received virtually no discussion in the literature. It is based on adjuncts of the form 'X or no X', where X is a nominal. The construction has been held to exemplify unbounded syntactic reduplication. We argue that although the argument can be made in a mathematically valid form, its empirical basis is not secure. First, the claimed unbounded syntactic identity between nominals does not always hold in attested cases, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • On the Necessity of U-Shaped Learning.Lorenzo Carlucci & John Case - 2013 - Topics in Cognitive Science 5 (1):56-88.
    A U-shaped curve in a cognitive-developmental trajectory refers to a three-step process: good performance followed by bad performance followed by good performance once again. U-shaped curves have been observed in a wide variety of cognitive-developmental and learning contexts. U-shaped learning seems to contradict the idea that learning is a monotonic, cumulative process and thus constitutes a challenge for competing theories of cognitive development and learning. U-shaped behavior in language learning (in particular in learning English past tense) has become a central (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Contexts and the Concept of Mild Context-Sensitivity.M. Kudlek, C. Martín-Vide, A. Mateescu & V. Mitrana - 2003 - Linguistics and Philosophy 26 (6):703 - 725.
    We introduce and study a natural extension of Marcus external contextual grammars. This mathematically simple mechanism which generates a proper subclass of simple matrix languages, known to be mildly context-sensitive ones, is still mildly context-sensitive. Furthermore, we get an infinite hierarchy of mildly context-sensitive families of languages. Then we attempt to fill a gap regarding the linguistic relevance of these mechanisms which consists in defining a tree structure on the strings generated by many-dimensional external contextual grammars, and investigate some related (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Systematicity and Natural Language Syntax.Barbara C. Scholz - 2007 - Croatian Journal of Philosophy 7 (3):375-402.
    A lengthy debate in the philosophy of the cognitive sciences has turned on whether the phenomenon known as ‘systematicity’ of language and thought shows that connectionist explanatory aspirations are misguided. We investigate the issue of just which phenomenon ‘systematicity’ is supposed to be. The much-rehearsed examples always suggest that being systematic has something to do with ways in which some parts of expressions in natural languages (and, more conjecturally, some parts of thoughts) can be substituted for others without altering well-formedness. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The concept of phrase structure.Alexis Manaster-Ramer & Michael B. Kac - 1990 - Linguistics and Philosophy 13 (3):325 - 362.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A computational representation for generalised phrase-structure grammars.John D. Phillips - 1992 - Linguistics and Philosophy 15 (3):255 - 287.
    Some modifications are suggested to recent (1985) generalised phrase-structure grammar which make the formalism more suitable to computational use, and at the same time provide a clear and elegant redefinition for parts of the formalism which are standardly complex and ill-defined. It is shown how the feature-instantiation principles can be represented as explicit rules in a format similar to metarules, and how a grammar of four parts, immediate-dominance rules, linear-precedence rules, metarules, and these new propagation rules, can be used to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “no crossing constraint” in autosegmental phonology.John Coleman & John Local - 1991 - Linguistics and Philosophy 14 (3):295 - 338.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Generalized quantifiers.Dag Westerståhl - 2008 - Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • The semantics of respective Readings, conjunction, and filler-gap dependencies.Jean Mark Gawron & Andrew Kehler - 2004 - Linguistics and Philosophy 27 (2):169-207.
    We provide a semantic analysis of respective readings, including butnot limited to the interpretation of examples containing the adverbrespectively, which accounts for a number of facts that haveeither proven difficult for previous studies or heretofore goneunnoticed in the literature. The analysis introduces the new notionsof property sum and proposition sum which integrate smoothly with existing analyses of plurals and distributivity. The analysis also admits of a straightforward account of previouslyunacknowledged examples involving filler-gap dependencies that areproblematic for contemporary syntactic theories. Ramifications (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • A linear precedence account of cross-serial dependencies.Almerindo E. Ojeda - 1988 - Linguistics and Philosophy 11 (4):457 - 492.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Do Humans Really Learn A n B n Artificial Grammars From Exemplars?Jean-Rémy Hochmann, Mahan Azadpour & Jacques Mehler - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (6):1021-1036.
    An important topic in the evolution of language is the kinds of grammars that can be computed by humans and other animals. Fitch and Hauser () approached this question by assessing the ability of different species to learn 2 grammars, (AB)n and An Bn. An Bn was taken to indicate a phrase structure grammar, eliciting a center‐embedded pattern. (AB)n indicates a grammar whose strings entail only local relations between the categories of constituents. F&H's data suggest that humans, but not tamarin (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Phrase structure parameters.Janet Dean Fodor & Stephen Crain - 1990 - Linguistics and Philosophy 13 (6):619-659.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unbounded syntactic copying in mandarin chinese.Daniel Radzinski - 1990 - Linguistics and Philosophy 13 (1):113 - 127.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Dutch as a formal language.Alexis Manaster-Ramer - 1987 - Linguistics and Philosophy 10 (2):221 - 246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Evidence against the context-freeness of natural language.Stuart M. Shieber - 1985 - Linguistics and Philosophy 8 (3):333 - 343.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   68 citations  
  • Scandinavian extraction phenomena revisited: Weak and strong generative capacity. [REVIEW]Philip H. Miller - 1991 - Linguistics and Philosophy 14 (1):101 - 113.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Phrase structure parameters.Janet Fodor & Stephen Crain - 1990 - Linguistics and Philosophy 13 (6):619 - 659.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A note onxy languages.Jerrold M. Sadock & Arnold M. Zwicky - 1985 - Linguistics and Philosophy 8 (2):229 - 236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Pāṇini's Grammar and Modern Computation.John Kadvany - 2016 - History and Philosophy of Logic 37 (4):325-346.
    Pāṇini's fourth century BC Sanskrit grammar uses rewrite rules utilizing an explicit formal language defined through a semi-formal metalanguage. The grammar is generative, meaning that it is capable of expressing a potential infinity of well-formed Sanskrit sentences starting from a finite symbolic inventory. The grammar's operational rules involve extensive use of auxiliary markers, in the form of Sanskrit phonemes, to control grammatical derivations. Pāṇini's rules often utilize a generic context-sensitive format to identify terms used in replacement, modification or deletion operations. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The complexity of the vocabulary of bambara.Christopher Culy - 1985 - Linguistics and Philosophy 8 (3):345 - 351.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations