Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. My behavior made me do it: The uncaused cause of teleological behaviorism.Jordan Hughes & Patricia S. Churchland - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):130-131.
    Toward a neurobiologically grounded approach to explaining self-control we discuss the case of a patient with a bilateral lesion in frontal ventromedial cortex. Patients with such lesions display a marked deficit in social decision making. Compared with an account that examines the causal antecedents of self-control, Rachlin's behaviorist approach seems lacking in explanatory strength.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preparatory response hypotheses: A muddle of causal and functional analyses.Karen L. Hollis - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):145-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-control as habit.Max Hocutt - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):129-130.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fears, phobias and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning.Arne Öhman & Susan Mineka - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (3):483-522.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   239 citations  
  • On the sufficiency of a Pavlovian conditioning model for coping with the complexities of neurosis.Arne Öhman & Holger Ursin - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):179-180.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The extended psychological present.Philip N. Hineline - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):128-129.
    Portraying psychological process as extended over time in multiply overlapping scales is a conceptual advance that can be understood as analogous to our understanding of spatial relationships. There may be a residual contradiction, however, when Rachlin invokes in ways that seem to imply earlier conceptions. The roles of superimposed or conditionally related stimuli also remain to be addressed.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Avoidance theory: Old wine, older bottles, a few new labels.Philip N. Hineline - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-680.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motivation and function.Robert W. Henderson - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):311-312.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Panksepp's psychobiological theory of emotions: Some substantiation.Robert G. Heath - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):432-433.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The multiplicity of physiological and behavioral variables modulating pain responses.Ronald L. Hayes - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):311-311.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Indicator semantics and Dretske's function.Valerie Gray Hardcastle - 1994 - Philosophical Psychology 7 (3):367-82.
    In his Explaining Behavior, Fred Dretske uses a reliabilist theory of representation to try to vindicate the use of intentional explanation for behaviour against latter-day elitninativism. Although Dretske's indicator semantics turns on the notion of function, he himself never explicitly defines what function means. Dretske's reticence in discussing function may ultimately be an error, for, as I argue, his implicit understanding of what a function amounts to does not fit with data from op rant conditioning. Still, this need not be (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reinforcement without representation.Stephen José Hanson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):141-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The effect of the magnitude of response-independent food on conditioned enhancement.Robert J. Hamm & Donald Meltzer - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 10 (4):339-342.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: The role of interdisciplinary theory.Stephen Grossberg - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):144-145.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Premature theorizing is not always parsimonious.Gary Greenberg - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):310-311.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Has learning been shown to be attractor modification within reinforcement modelling?Robert A. M. Gregson - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):140-141.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Choice between long- and short-term interests: Beyond self-control.Leonard Green & Joel Myerson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):127-128.
    In the real world, there are choices between large, delayed, punctate rewards and small, more immediate rewards as well as choices between patterns and acts. A common element in these situations is the choice between long- and short-term interests. Key issues for future research appear to be how acts are restructured into larger patterns of behavior, and whether, as Rachlin implies, pattern perception is the cause of pattern generation.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the classification of the emotions.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):431-432.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • On the difference between pain and fear.Jeffrey A. Gray - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):310-310.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Is there any need for conditioning in Eysenck's conditioning model of neurosis?Jeffrey A. Gray - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):169-171.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Irrelevant-incentive learning and two-process theory.Douglas S. Grant, Sheila M. Greer & Donald D. Severance - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 14 (4):297-300.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some distinctions among representations.M. Gopnik - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):378-379.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Can phenomenology contribute to brain science?Gordon G. Globus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):430-431.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond Pavlovian classical conditioning.Beatrix T. Gardner & R. Allen Gardner - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Moving beyond schedules and rate: A new trajectory?Gregory Galbicka - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):139-140.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Flights of teleological fancy about classical conditioning do not produce valid science or useful technology.John J. Furedy - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Internal commitment and efficient habit formation.Robert H. Frank - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):127-127.
    Rachlin's attack on the internal commitment model rests on the demonstrably false claim that self-punishment does not exist. He is correct that habits are an effective device for solving self-control problems, but his additional claim that they are the only such device makes it hard to explain how good habits develop in the first place. Someone with a self-control problem would always choose the spuriously attractive reward, which, over time, would create bad habits.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pain and fear are different motivations.Elzbieta Fonberg - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):308-310.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Emotions are objective events.Elzbieta Fonberg - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):429-430.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pain is sufficient to activate the endorphin-mediated analgesia system.Howard L. Fields - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):308-308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Explaining classical conditioning: Phenomenological unity conceals mechanistic diversity.Chris Fields - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):141-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The future is uncertain: Eat dessert first.Edmund Fantino - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):125-126.
    There may be evolutionary as well as economic reasons why organisms generally act impulsively. I discuss this possibility and suggest some follow-up experiments that may clarify the exciting empirical and theoretical contributions made by the experiments discussed in the target article.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Response utility in classical and operant conditioning.Edmund Fantino - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):141-141.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The conditioning theory of neurosis: criticisms considered.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):188-199.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The conditioning model of neurosis.H. J. Eysenck - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):155-166.
    The long-term persistence of neurotic symptoms, such as anxiety, poses difficult problems for any psychological theory. An attempt is made to revive the Watson-Mowrer conditioning theory and to avoid the many criticisms directed against it in the past. It is suggested that recent research has produced changes in learning theory that can be used to render this possible. In the first place, the doctrine of equipotentiality has been shown to be wrong, and some such concept as Seligman's “preparedness” is required, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   96 citations  
  • Fear, pain, and arousal.H. J. Eysenck - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):307-308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Representation: A concept that fills no gaps.Robert Epstein - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):377-378.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Does behaviorism explain self-control?Robert Eisenberger - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):125-125.
    Rachlin's hyperbolic-discounting model captures basic features of the subtlety of human impulsiveness and self-control and has received convincing experimental support. His distinction between self-control patterns and impulsive acts expands his earlier work to a greater range of self-control behaviors. Possible mechanisms that may weaken or strengthen patterns of self-control are considered.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Fear and pain: semantic, biochemical and clinical reflections.Burr Eichelman - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):306-307.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anticipatory affect during action preparation: evidence from backward compatibility in dual-task performance.Andreas B. Eder, Roland Pfister, David Dignath & Bernhard Hommel - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (6):1211-1224.
    Upcoming responses in the second of two subsequently performed tasks can speed up compatible responses in the temporally preceding first task. Two experiments extend previous demonstration of such backward compatibility to affective features: responses to affective stimuli were faster in Task 1 when an affectively compatible response effect was anticipated for Task 2. This emotional backward-compatibility effect demonstrates that representations of the affective consequences of the Task 2 response were activated before the selection of a response in Task 1 was (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Gantt and Eysenck conditioning models for neurosis.Roscoe A. Dykman - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-169.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning and functional utility.Barry R. Dworkin - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):139-141.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Naloxone produces a fear and pain model.Ronald Dubner - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):306-306.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The informational character of representations.Fred Dretske - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):376-377.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • What do reinforcers strengthen? The unit of selection.John W. Donahoe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):138-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Conditioning of sexual and reproductive behavior: Extending the hegemony to the propagation of species.Michael Domjan & Susan Nash - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):138-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is this defense needed?James A. Dinsmoor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):679-679.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How absent negativity relates to affect and motivation: an integrative relief model.Roland Deutsch, Kevin J. M. Smith, Robert Kordts-Freudinger & Regina Reichardt - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Introspection as the Rosetta stone: Millstone or fifth wheel?Ronald de Sousa - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):428-429.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark