Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Which cognitive tools do we prefer to use, and is that preference rational?Boris Alexandre, Jordan Navarro, Emanuelle Reynaud & François Osiurak - 2019 - Cognition 186:108-114.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Googled Assertion.J. Adam Carter & Emma C. Gordon - 2017 - Philosophical Psychology 30 (4):490-501.
    Recent work in the philosophy of mind and cognitive science (e.g., Clark and Chalmers 1998; Clark 2010a; Clark 2010b; Palermos 2014) can help to explain why certain kinds of assertions—made on the basis of information stored in our gadgets rather than in biological memory—are properly criticisable in light of misleading implicatures, while others are not.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Problem Solvers Adjust Cognitive Offloading Based on Performance Goals.Patrick P. Weis & Eva Wiese - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (12):e12802.
    When incorporating the environment into mental processing (cf., cognitive offloading), one creates novel cognitive strategies that have the potential to improve task performance. Improved performance can, for example, mean faster problem solving, more accurate solutions, or even higher grades at university.1 Although cognitive offloading has frequently been associated with improved performance, it is yet unclear how flexible problem solvers are at matching their offloading habits with their current performance goals (can people improve goal‐related instead of generic performance, e.g., when being (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Arranging Objects in Space: Measuring Task‐Relevant Organizational Behaviors During Goal Pursuit.Grayden J. F. Solman & Alan Kingstone - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (4):1042-1070.
    Human behavior unfolds primarily in built environments, where the arrangement of objects is a result of ongoing human decisions and actions, yet these organizational decisions have received limited experimental study. In two experiments, we introduce a novel paradigm designed to explore how individuals organize task-relevant objects in space. Participants completed goals by locating and accessing sequences of objects in a computer-based task, and they were free to rearrange the positions of objects at any time. We measure a variety of organization (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Storing information in-the-world: Metacognition and cognitive offloading in a short-term memory task.Evan F. Risko & Timothy L. Dunn - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 36:61-74.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Offloading memory leaves us vulnerable to memory manipulation.E. F. Risko, M. O. Kelly, P. Patel & C. Gaspar - 2019 - Cognition 191 (C):103954.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Answers at your fingertips: Access to the Internet influences willingness to answer questions.Amanda M. Ferguson, David McLean & Evan F. Risko - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 37:91-102.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Toward a Metacognitive Account of Cognitive Offloading.Timothy L. Dunn & Evan F. Risko - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (5):1080-1127.
    Individuals frequently make use of the body and environment when engaged in a cognitive task. For example, individuals will often spontaneously physically rotate when faced with rotated objects, such as an array of words, to putatively offload the performance costs associated with stimulus rotation. We looked to further examine this idea by independently manipulating the costs associated with both word rotation and array frame rotation. Surprisingly, we found that individuals’ patterns of spontaneous physical rotations did not follow patterns of performance (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations