Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Failure to form a learned taste aversion in rats with amygdaloid lesions.Ernest D. Kemble & Jennifer A. Nagel - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 2 (3):155-156.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A fourth approach to the study of learning: Are “processes” really necessary?John C. Malone - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):151-152.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • On the what and how of learning.R. C. Gonzalez & Matthew Yarczower - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):145-145.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Arlo K. Myers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):681-682.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Proto-, pre-, and pro-intelligence: Little evidence but a necessary assumption.Randolf Menzel - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):674.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The epistemology of intelligence: Contextual variables, tautologies, and external referents.Craig T. Nagoshi - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):675.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Boiling down intelligence.Alison Jolly - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):671.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The several meanings of intelligence.H. J. Eysenck - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):663.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning beyond the reflex: An uneasy rebirth.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):161-179.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Classical conditioning and the placebo effect.Ian Wickram - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):160-161.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mis-representations.J. Bruce Overmier - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):156-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pattern proliferation in teleological behaviorism.Bruce N. Waller - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):145-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Expectancy bias and phobias: Accounting for the uneven distribution of fears and the characteristics of clinical phobias.Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):315-325.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Phobias and anxiety in the framework of the defense reflex.E. N. Sokolov - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):313-313.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Responses conditioned to fear-relevant stimuli survive extinction of the expectancy of the UCS.Anne M. Schell & Michael E. Dawson - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):312-313.
    Davey suggests that increased resistance to extinction of CRs conditioned to fear-relevant stimuli may be due to more persistent expectancies of the UCS following these stimuli. However, this viewpoint is contradicted by existing empirical evidence that fear-relevant CRs survive an extinction trials series producing extinction of expectancies whereas CRs conditioned to non-fear-relevant CSs do not.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Preparedness, phobias, and the Panglossian paradigm.Richard J. McNally - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):303-304.
    In his critique of preparedness theory, Davey does not address the limitations of adaptationism. The purpose of this commentary is to outline problems that arise when one assumes that mental illness (e.g., phobic disorder)musthave had adaptive significance for it to have survived the vicissitudes of natural selection.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Expectancy bias as sole or partial account of selective associations?Susan Mineka & Michael Cook - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):307-309.
    Davey reviews evidence purporting to distinguish between two accounts of selective associations – expectancy bias and evolved predispositions, although these hypotheses largely apply to different levels of causal analysis. Criticisms of primate studies in which subjects lack prior exposure to stimuli seem uncompelling. Expectancies may sometimes serve as proximal mediators in selective associations, but other factors, both proximate and ultimate, are clearly also involved.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Preparedness and phobias: Specific evolved associations or a generalized expectancy bias?Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):289-297.
    Most phobias are focussed on a small number of fear-inducing stimuli (e.g., snakes, spiders). A review of the evidence supporting biological and cognitive explanations of this uneven distribution of phobias suggests that the readiness with which such stimuli become associated with aversive outcomes arises from biases in the processing of information about threatening stimuli rather than from phylogenetically based associative predispositions or “biological preparedness.” This cognitive bias, consisting of a heightened expectation of aversive outcomes following fear-relevant stimuli, generates and maintains (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Disgust: Sensory affect or primary emotional system?Judith A. Toronchuk & George Fr Ellis - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (8):1799-1818.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Self-control: Beyond commitment.Howard Rachlin - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):109-121.
    Self-control, so important in the theory and practice of psychology, has usually been understood introspectively. This target article adopts a behavioral view of the self (as an abstract class of behavioral actions) and of self-control (as an abstract behavioral pattern dominating a particular act) according to which the development of self-control is a molar/molecular conflict in the development of behavioral patterns. This subsumes the more typical view of self-control as a now/later conflict in which an act of self-control is a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   122 citations  
  • Commitment: Beyond Rachlin's control?N. E. Wetherick - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):146-147.
    Rachlin's view of self-control is rejected on the grounds that his arguments do not establish the possibility of abstract, external, stimulus patterns and that his experiments, although they show that pigeons and human beings do sometimes choose postponed rather than immediate gratification, do not challenge the commonly held view that internal factors are involved in the former choice.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Encounter processes, prey densities, and efficient diets.Thomas Caraco - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):333-334.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Reaching for an integrated science of behavior.Clifton Lee Gass - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):337-337.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Preference for a hypothesis: Is the case “closed”?Marc N. Branch - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):332-333.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • In defense of nativism.Eric Margolis & Stephen Laurence - 2013 - Philosophical Studies 165 (2):693-718.
    This paper takes a fresh look at the nativism–empiricism debate, presenting and defending a nativist perspective on the mind. Empiricism is often taken to be the default view both in philosophy and in cognitive science. This paper argues, on the contrary, that there should be no presumption in favor of empiricism (or nativism), but that the existing evidence suggests that nativism is the most promising framework for the scientific study of the mind. Our case on behalf of nativism has four (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Modularity, and the psychoevolutionary theory of emotion.Paul E. Griffiths - 1990 - Biology and Philosophy 5 (2):175-196.
    It is unreasonable to assume that our pre-scientific emotion vocabulary embodies all and only those distinctions required for a scientific psychology of emotion. The psychoevolutionary approach to emotion yields an alternative classification of certain emotion phenomena. The new categories are based on a set of evolved adaptive responses, or affect-programs, which are found in all cultures. The triggering of these responses involves a modular system of stimulus appraisal, whose evoluations may conflict with those of higher-level cognitive processes. Whilst the structure (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • The challenge of green marketing communication.Carmen Grebmer - 2020 - Dissertation, Ludwig Maximilians Universität, München
    Understanding how packaging communication guides consumers in evaluating the environmental performance of a product is essential to promoting sustainable consumption. Previous studies suggest that while consumers are unable to verify the veracity of environmental information and the actual environmental impact of a product, they use packaging communication to evaluate packaging and product quality subjectively. However, few studies focus on the aspects of efficient and credible green marketing communication and the role of communication channels used. This situation applies, in particular, consumers (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning.Michael Waldmann (ed.) - 2017 - Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    Causal reasoning is one of our most central cognitive competencies, enabling us to adapt to our world. Causal knowledge allows us to predict future events, or diagnose the causes of observed facts. We plan actions and solve problems using knowledge about cause-effect relations. Without our ability to discover and empirically test causal theories, we would not have made progress in various empirical sciences. In the past decades, the important role of causal knowledge has been discovered in many areas of cognitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • (1 other version)Are species intelligent?: Not a yes or no question.Jonathan Schull - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):94-108.
    Plant and animal species are information-processing entities of such complexity, integration, and adaptive competence that it may be scientifically fruitful to consider them intelligent. The possibility arises from the analogy between learning and evolution, and from recent developments in evolutionary science, psychology and cognitive science. Species are now described as spatiotemporally localized individuals in an expanded hierarchy of biological entities. Intentional and cognitive abilities are now ascribed to animal, human, and artificial intelligence systems that process information adaptively, and that manifest (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Thyme to touch: Infants possess strategies that protect them from dangers posed by plants.Annie E. Wertz & Karen Wynn - 2014 - Cognition 130 (1):44-49.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Core knowledge and its limits: The domain of food.Kristin Shutts, Kirsten F. Condry, Laurie R. Santos & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2009 - Cognition 112 (1):120-140.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Are species intelligent? Look for genetic knowledge structures.J. David Smith - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):89-90.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Homeostasis and drinking.F. M. Toates - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):95-102.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  • Learning theory in its niche.Howard Rachlin - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):155-156.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • The nature of learning explanations.John Garcia - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Avoidance behavior: Assumptions, theory, and metatheory.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-696.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Advances toward a biological theory of aversive learning: Flirtation or commitment?Dallas Treit & Marcia L. Spetch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):684-685.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Natural selection and intelligence.David F. Sherry - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):678.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Within-species variations in g: The case of Homo sapiens.John G. Borkowski - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):660.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Compensatory behaviors and the “rest principle”.John David Sinclair - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):466-466.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The modelling of incentive motivation processes.Frederick M. Toates - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (3):466-468.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Classical conditioning: The role of interdisciplinary theory.Stephen Grossberg - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):144-145.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The importance of classical conditioning.H. D. Kimmel - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):148-149.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classical conditioning: The new hegemony.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):121-137.
    Converging data from different disciplines are showing the role of classical conditioning processes in the elaboration of human and animal behavior to be larger than previously supposed. Restricted views of classically conditioned responses as merely secretory, reflexive, or emotional are giving way to a broader conception that includes problem-solving, and other rule-governed behavior thought to be the exclusive province of either operant conditiońing or cognitive psychology. These new views have been accompanied by changes in the way conditioning is conducted and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • Choice and preference-you can't always want what you get.Alasdair I. Houston - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):339-340.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Does behaviorism explain self-control?Robert Eisenberger - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):125-125.
    Rachlin's hyperbolic-discounting model captures basic features of the subtlety of human impulsiveness and self-control and has received convincing experimental support. His distinction between self-control patterns and impulsive acts expands his earlier work to a greater range of self-control behaviors. Possible mechanisms that may weaken or strengthen patterns of self-control are considered.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The behavior of self-control.Joseph J. Plaud - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):139-140.
    Rachlin's view of self-control as a sequence or chain of behaviors is contrasted with traditional behavioral analyses of self-control which emphasize a simplistic interpretation of the hyperbolic function relating small-sooner (SS) and larger-later (LL) reinforcers to specific behaviors. The validity of Rachlin's teleological analysis is examined in relation to the acquisition and steady-state performance of self-control behaviors. Central to an analysis of self-control is the functional difference between behavior under the control of SS and LL reinforcers, because SS-reinforced behavior is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Conceptual problems in the act-versus-pattern analysis of self-control.Suresh Kanekar - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):132-133.
    The primary argument against Rachlin's act-versus-pattern analysis of self-control is that it is wrong to think of a temptation as a solitary act while the alternative is conceived of as an element of a pattern. Either both are solitary acts or both are members of patterns, however different the patterns may be in their complexity and abstractness.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Alternative approaches to the psychology of foraging.John M. Kruse - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):342-343.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations