Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Discovering and training the components of intelligence.Colin M. MacLeod - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):597-598.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The epistemological import of morphological content.Jack C. Lyons - 2014 - Philosophical Studies 169 (3):537-547.
    Morphological content (MC) is content that is implicit in the standing structure of the cognitive system. Henderson and Horgan claim that MC plays a distinctive epistemological role unrecognized by traditional epistemic theories. I consider the possibilities that MC plays this role either in central cognition or in peripheral modules. I argue that the peripheral MC does not play an interesting epistemological role and that the central MC is already recognized by traditional theories.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Algorithm and Parameters: Solving the Generality Problem for Reliabilism.Jack C. Lyons - 2019 - Philosophical Review 128 (4):463-509.
    The paper offers a solution to the generality problem for a reliabilist epistemology, by developing an “algorithm and parameters” scheme for type-individuating cognitive processes. Algorithms are detailed procedures for mapping inputs to outputs. Parameters are psychological variables that systematically affect processing. The relevant process type for a given token is given by the complete algorithmic characterization of the token, along with the values of all the causally relevant parameters. The typing that results is far removed from the typings of folk (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Conscious identification: Where do you draw the line?Stephen J. Lupker - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):37-38.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Is the social chunking of agent actions in working memory resource-demanding?Xiqian Lu, Alessandro Dai, Yang Guo, Mowei Shen & Zaifeng Gao - 2022 - Cognition 229 (C):105249.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Schizophrenia and attention: In and out of context.R. E. Lubow - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):35-36.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Human consciousness: One of a kind.R. E. Lubow - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):689-689.
    To avoid teleological interpretations, it is important to make a distinction between functions and uses of consciousness, and to address questions concerning the consequences of consciousness. Assumptions about the phylogenetic distribution of consciousness are examined. It is concluded that there is some value in identifying consciousness an exclusively human attribute.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Broadbent's Maltese cross memory model: Something old, something new, something borrowed, something missing.Elizabeth F. Loftus, Geoffrey R. Loftus & Earl B. Hunt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):73-74.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Correlating mind and body.T. J. Lioyd-Jones, N. Donnelly & B. Weekes - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):688-688.
    Gray's integration of the different levels of description and explanation in his theory is problematic: The introduction of consciousness into his theorising consists of the mind-brain identity assumption, which tells us nothing new. There need not be correlations between levels of description. Gray's account does not extend beyond “brute” correlation. Integration must be achieved in a principled, mutually constraining way.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tacit knowledge and verbal report: On sinking ships and saving babies.R. O. Lindsay & B. Gorayska - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):410-411.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attentional theories and conscious perception.Benjamin Libet - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):247-248.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What Klein’s “Semantic Gradient” Does and Does Not Really Show: Decomposing Stroop Interference into Task and Informational Conflict Components.Yulia Levin & Joseph Tzelgov - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Simplifying heuristics versus careful thinking: Scientific analysis of millennial spiritual issues.Daniel S. Levine & Leonid I. Perlovsky - 2008 - Zygon 43 (4):797-821.
    There is ample evidence that humans (and other primates) possess a knowledge instinct—a biologically driven impulse to make coherent sense of the world at the highest level possible. Yet behavioral decision-making data suggest a contrary biological drive to minimize cognitive effort by solving problems using simplifying heuristics. Individuals differ, and the same person varies over time, in the strength of the knowledge instinct. Neuroimaging studies suggest which brain regions might mediate the balance between knowledge expansion and heuristic simplification. One region (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Simplifying Heuristics Versus Careful Thinking: Scientific Analysis of Millennial Spiritual Issues.Daniel S. Levine & Leonid I. Perlovsky - 2008 - Zygon 43 (4):797-821.
    Abstract.There is ample evidence that humans (and other primates) possess a knowledge instinct—a biologically driven impulse to make coherent sense of the world at the highest level possible. Yet behavioral decision‐making data suggest a contrary biological drive to minimize cognitive effort by solving problems using simplifying heuristics. Individuals differ, and the same person varies over time, in the strength of the knowledge instinct. Neuroimaging studies suggest which brain regions might mediate the balance between knowledge expansion and heuristic simplification. One region (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • When explanation is too hard (or understanding hijacking for novices).Michael Lebowitz - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):662-663.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Approaches to consciousness: Psychophysics or philosophy?Richard Latto & John Campion - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):36-37.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Models of Competence in Solving Physics Problems.Jill H. Larkin, John McDermott, Dorothea P. Simon & Herbert A. Simon - 1980 - Cognitive Science 4 (4):317-345.
    We describe a set of two computer‐implemented models that solve physics problems in ways characteristic of more and less competent human solvers. The main features accounting for different competences are differences in strategy for selecting physics principles, and differences in the degree of automation in the process of applying a single principle. The models provide a good account of the order in which principles are applied by human solvers working problems in kinematics and dynamics. They also are sufficiently flexible to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   56 citations  
  • New failures to learn.Barbara Landau - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):660-661.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Matters of mind: Mindfulness/mindlessness in perspective.E. J. Langer - 1992 - Consciousness and Cognition 1 (3):289-305.
    The dual concepts of mindfulness and mindlessness are described. Mindfulness is a state of conscious awareness in which the individual is implicitly aware of the context and content of information. It is a state of openness to novelty in which the individual actively constructs categories and distinctions. In contrast, mindlessness is a state of mind characterized by an over reliance on categories and distinctions drawn in the past and in which the individual is context-dependent and, as such, is oblivious to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Intelligence: Toward a modern sketch of a good g.Herbert Lansdell - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):597-597.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Induction and explanation: Complementary models of learning.Pat Langley - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):661-662.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): Still no identification without attention.Joel Lachter, Kenneth I. Forster & Eric Ruthruff - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (4):880-913.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Consciousness in natural language and motor learning.Joel Lachter - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):409-410.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Induction and probability.Henry E. Kyburg - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):660-660.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Functional architecture and free will.Henry E. Kyburg - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):143-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The cognitive map overlaps the environmental frame, the situation, and the real-world formulary.Benjamin Kuipers - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):298-299.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of priming in conjunctive visual search.Árni Kristjánsson, DeLiang Wang & Ken Nakayama - 2002 - Cognition 85 (1):37-52.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Similarities between attentional and preparatory states.Rumyana Kristeva & Douglas Cheyne - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):247-247.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults.Arthur F. Kramer, John F. Larish & David L. Strayer - 1995 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 1 (1):50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Responding to attention.K. Kranda - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):762-762.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modelling attention in man.K. Kranda - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):246-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • On what it means to automatize a rule.Paul Kovacs & F. Gregory Ashby - 2022 - Cognition 226 (C):105168.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Can procedural learning be equated with unconscious learning or rule-based learning?Zoe Kourtzi, Lindsay M. Oliver & Mark A. Gluck - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):408-409.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Positive mood boosts the expression of a dispositional need for closure.Małgorzata Kossowska & Yoram Bar-Tal - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (7):1181-1201.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Connectionism: There's something to it.Stephen M. Kosslyn, Scott D. Mainwaring & Thomas A. Corcoran - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):297-298.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction Framework: Bridging the Science-Practice Chasm to Enhance Robust Student Learning.Kenneth R. Koedinger, Albert T. Corbett & Charles Perfetti - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (5):757-798.
    Despite the accumulation of substantial cognitive science research relevant to education, there remains confusion and controversy in the application of research to educational practice. In support of a more systematic approach, we describe the Knowledge-Learning-Instruction (KLI) framework. KLI promotes the emergence of instructional principles of high potential for generality, while explicitly identifying constraints of and opportunities for detailed analysis of the knowledge students may acquire in courses. Drawing on research across domains of science, math, and language learning, we illustrate the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Problem solving as a cognitive process.Manfred Kochen - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):599-600.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Factors or processes in intelligence.Paul Kline - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):596-597.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The elusive visual processing mode: Implications of the architecture/algorithm distinction.Roberta L. Klatzky - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Role of affective associations in the planning and habit systems of decision-making related to addiction.Marc T. Kiviniemi & Rick A. Bevins - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (4):450-451.
    The model proposed by Redish et al. considers vulnerabilities within decision systems based on expectancy-value assumptions. Further understanding of processes leading to addiction can be gained by considering other inputs to decision-making, particularly affective associations with behaviors. This consideration suggests additional decision-making vulnerabilities that might explain addictive behaviors.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Second-generation AI theories of learning.David Kirsh - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):658-659.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Descartes' psychology of vision and cognitive science: The optics (1637) in the light of Marr's (1982) vision.Geir kirkebøen - 1998 - Philosophical Psychology 11 (2):161 – 182.
    In this paper I consider the relation between Descartes' psychology of vision and the cognitive science approach to psychology (henceforth CS). In particular, I examine Descartes' the Optics (1637) in the light of David Marr's (1982) position in CS. My general claim is that CS can be seen as a rediscovery of Descartes' psychology of vision. In the first section, I point to a parallel between Descartes' epistemological revolution, which created the modem version of the problem of perception, and the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Septohippocampal comparator: Consciousness generator or attention feedback loop?Marcel Kinsbourne - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):687-688.
    As Gray insists, his comparator model proposes a brute correlation only – of consciousness with septohippocampal output. I suggest that the comparator straddles a feedback loop that boosts the activation ofnovelrepresentations, thus helping them feature in present or recollected experience. Such a role in organizing conscious contents would transcend correlation and help explain how consciousness emerges from brain function.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Human autonomic conditioning without awareness.H. D. Kimmel - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):408-408.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attentional modulation of masked semantic priming by visible and masked task cues.Markus Kiefer, Natalie M. Trumpp, Caroline Schaitz, Heiko Reuss & Wilfried Kunde - 2019 - Cognition 187 (C):62-77.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Sensitivity of different measures of the visibility of masked primes: Influences of prime–response and prime–target relations.Shah Khalid, Peter König & Ulrich Ansorge - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1473-1488.
    Visual masking of primes lowers prime visibility but spares processing of primes as reflected in prime–target congruence and prime–response compatibility effects. However, the question is how to appropriately measure prime visibility. Here, we tested the influence of three procedural variables on prime visibility measures: prime–target similarity, prime–response similarity, and the variability of prime–response mappings. Our results show that a low prime–target similarity is a favorable condition for a prime visibility measure because it increases the sensitivity of this measure in comparison (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The category effect in visual selective attention.Patti L. Kelly, David W. Harrison & Milton H. Hodge - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (1):71-74.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Clarity, generality, and efficiency in models of learning: Wringing the MOP.Kevin T. Kelly - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):657-658.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reductionism and cognitive flexibility.Frank Keil - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (1):141-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sternberg's sketchy theory: Defining details desired.Daniel P. Keating - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):595-596.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations